Correlation between end-weight principle and adjective
clauses in English: Q1 SCOPUS applied linguistics
articles and Thai EFL learners essays
Abhinan Wongkittiporn
English Language Department, Rangsit University, Thailand.
Accepted 26 March, 2024
ABSTRACT
The researcher in this study applied the notions of quantitative method, theoretical linguistics and applied
linguistics to examine the correlation between the linguistic theory of end-weight principle and English
adjective clauses in Q1-SCOPUS applied linguistics articles and Thai EFL learners descriptive essays. The
significance of this study is that structure and use in writing should be learned simultaneously. This current
study also innovatively supports Thai EFL learners to examine their linguistic competence in structuring
information in their English writing. The data collection in this study was divided into two datasets. The first
dataset was 20 Q1-SCOPUS applied linguistics articles published between 2022 and 2024. Approximately
200,000 words provided 117 sentences containing adjective clauses in the final position or VP-
postmodifiers. On the other hand, the second dataset was 67 descriptive essays written by Thai EFL high-
school learners from both public schools and private schools under the supervision of the Ministry of
Education, Thailand. The original theory of the end-weight principle proposed by Quirk et al. (1972) is
followed in this study. The syntactic structure of adjective clauses follows Radford (2009) who analyzed
adjective clauses as complementizer phrases (CP). The inferential statistical analysis of correlation follows
SPSS 29. The results showed that there was a statistically significant relationship between the datasets of
applied linguistics articles and EFL learners descriptive essays where the p-values were reported at 0.01
and 0.02, respectively. Not only do these statistical relationships show that English is an end-weight
language, but also Thai EFL learners can follow the norm accurately. This is due to the positive effect of the
mother tongue interference since the Thai language is an end-weight language. The pedagogical
implication of the study to EFL learners is that information structures should be contributed to grammar
classes. For future studies, it is recommended to test the hypothesis of the end-weight principle with
reduced adjective clauses in the medial position of sentences.
Keywords: Adjective clauses, applied linguistics articles, descriptive essays, end-weight principle, Thai
EFL learners.
*Corresponding author. E-mail: abhinan.w@rsu.ac.th.
INTRODUCTION
Information structure is an important linguistic aspect of
arranging information into clauses, sentences and
paragraphs (Birner and Ward, 2009). It is viewed as
similar to that of building constructions that connect
everything. However, most English language learners are
apt to ignore studying information structure. They seem to
pay more attention to the study of vocabulary and
grammar in writing (Wongkittiporn, 2024). Although the
notion of vocabulary and grammar could help them write
to communicate, the way information is linked to creating
African Educational Research Journal
Vol. 12(1), pp. 67-77, March 2024
DOI: 10.30918/AERJ.121.24.016
ISSN: 2354-2160
Full Length Research Paper
Afr Educ Res J 68
the smoothness of reading seems problematic.
The English Language is heavy-ended. This means
that the head is short, while the longer part is left for the
final position of the sentence. In other words, writing a
long subject is not uncommon in English, such as (1a).
(1)
(a) To swim for 50 minutes every day is good.
(b) It is good to swim for 50 minutes every day.
Examples (1a)-(1b) are arbitrary control constructions,
which are factual or habitual information applicable to
everyone. Even though both examples are semantically
the same, the way the information is structured is
different between the two examples. Example (1a) shows
the heaviness of the subject. On the other hand, example
(1b) shows that the expletive it is the shorter DP which is
dropped at the Spec T or the subject position (Radford,
2009). When making a comparison, this explains why
example (1b) is a more common structure in English due
to the end-weight principle.
Since theory plays an important role in English writing,
literature shows that there are three main camps of
researchers who applied the end-weight principle to study
written texts in different materials. The first group of
researchers used the theory to study the written texts of
novels and poems (Nofal, 2012). The long and heavy
information on VP-postmodifiers in novels gives a climax
to the story. The second group of researchers applied the
theory to study ditransitive verbs in international corpora
(Arnold et al., 2000; Ottmar, 2015). The results showed
the NP shift to the final position mostly at 70 percent. The
third group of researchers applied the end-weight
principle to study EFL learners writing (Lozano and
Mayamendikoetxea, 2009). This group is so interesting
as the results show that only advanced EFL learners, or
about C1 level, applied the end-weight principle correctly.
Whilst previous studies focused on the end-weight
principle to study novels, international corpora, and
advanced EFL writing, this study innovatively contributes
to the field by using SCOPUS-Q1 applied linguistics
research articles and Thai EFL learners descriptive
essays.
The ways that information structures in applied
linguistics articles are structured are regarded as effective
examples for novice researchers and graduate students
in the field. Today, research is a part of education in every
degree. We are taught what research is, but not everyone
is taught how to write research. Consequently, both
native and non-native English students struggle to
structure information in their research writing. This study
provides guidelines and examples of information
structures for new researchers entering the field. This
study also innovatively encourages Thai EFL learners to
examine their linguistic competence in structuring
information in English. This information leads one to the
following objective of the study.
Objectives of the study
1. To investigate the correlation between the end-weight
principle and English adjective clauses in Q1-SCOPUS
applied linguistics articles
2. To investigate the correlation between the end-weight
principle and English adjective clauses in Thai EFL
Mattayom learners descriptive essays
Hypotheses of the study
1. There is no significant relationship between the end-
weight principle and English adjective clauses in Q1
SCOPUS applied linguistics articles.
2. There is no significant relationship between the end-
weight principle and adjective clauses in Thai EFL
Mattayom learners descriptive essays.
LITERATURE REVIEW
Conceptual framework
This section shows the conceptual framework in this
study. The conceptual framework in this study is divided
into independent variables and dependent variables. The
independent variable in this study is the theoretical
framework of the end-weight principle (Quirk et al., 1972).
On the other hand, the dependent variables are adjective
clauses and the materials of applied linguistics research
articles and Thai EFL learners descriptive essays. Figure
1 shows how independent variables in this study were
investigated using the end-weight principle as presented
in concordance lines.
investigated using end
-
weight principle as presented in concordance lines.
(2)
(a) XXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXX, who OOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOO
(b) XXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXX, which OOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOO
(c) XXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXX, that OOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOO
(d) XXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXX, whose OOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOO
Figure
1
Concordance Lines
Figure 1. Concordance lines.
Wongkittiporn 69
Figure 1 shows concordance lines of how information is
keyed. The semiotics XXX is the main clause. On the
other hand, the semiotics OOO represents adjective
clauses. This study is interested in whether the OOO part
is linguistically expressed with adjective clauses or
reduced adjective clauses. English is a heavy-end
language. It is assumed the adjective clauses are the
structures of the final position of the sentence.
Theoretical background of the end-weight principle
Historically, the end-weight principle originated from the
Prague School of Linguistics in the 1920s. The school
integrated the philosophies of structuralism and
functionalism into linguistic study (Pribylová, 2019). End-
weight principle, sometimes called grammatical weight
(Wasaw, 1997) or the short-before-long ordering principle
is a theory in the branch of information structure (Aarts,
2011). The end-weight principle refers to the placement of
shorter information at the beginning and the longer and
heavier information at the final position of the sentence.
Quirk et al. (1972) was the first scholar who termed the
heaviness of constructions in the final position as end-
weight as follows:
[…] One is the tendency to place new
information toward the end of the clause,
and another is the tendency to reserve the
final position for the more complex parts of
a clause or sentence. (Quirk et al., 1972,
p. 766)
In English, a sentence usually conveys one main topic
and the topic of the sentence is short and written in the
subject position. In English, the topic, focus and important
information are generally provided at topicalization or the
syntactic position of spec T, referring to the subject
position. On the other hand, the longer information is a
post-verbal modifier, which is placed at the final position
of the sentence, such as adjuncts, temporal adverbs and
complements. This principle cognitively helps reduce the
workload of the brain in remembering the information at
the beginning of the sentence (Aarts, 2011). Basically, the
topic should be given before elaborating.
If a sentence in the English language is divided into two
parts which are pre-verbal and post-verbal, the pre-verbal
position is the position of the subject. Although the
subject carries the most important position of the
sentence, the information in this position should be
lighter, and less informative than the post-verbal section
(Smolka, 2017).
(3) Running improves the metabolism, blood circulation
and digestive system.
Example (3) shows how to structure information pre-
verbally and post-verbally. The pre-verbal position
running is lighter and contains only the topic. The post-
verbal position is the elaboration of this topic. When we
elaborate, we are likely to give longer details to support
the readers’ understanding of the topic (Smolka, 2017).
When compared with written language, the end-weight
principle is more common than spoken register as the
subject in this register tends to be short, such as the
pronouns I and you.
End-weight principle supports the ease of reading the
text (Nofal, 2012). Postponing the longer information,
such as adjective clauses and adverbial clauses could
help the readers to understand the text more easily.
Wasaw (1997) suggested that the end-weight principle
is for the benefit of information preparation or information
planning. Using the end-weight principle shows that the
speakers prepare information effectively. To prove this
statement, Wasaw (1997) analyzed approximately 800
sentences of the following collocation: attribute... to,
bring... to, and obtain... from, share... with. The results
show that approximately 55 percent are fixed colligations
as in obtain from and share with, such as (4).
(4)
(a) Pat brought a box with a ribbon around it to the party.
(b) Pat brought to the party a box with a ribbon around it.
(Wasaw, 1997, p. 355)
Example (4b) shows the colligation of brought to or the
use of verb and preposition in an adjacent area. The
information the party a box with a ribbon around it was
left at the final position in accordance with the end-weight
principle.
Generative grammar of adjective clauses in English
Adjective clause in English is structurally known as a DP
post-modifier. It gives additional or extra information
about its head, which is expressed in the determiner
phrase DP, such as the man, the animal and the object.
These DPs could be either the subject or the object of the
sentence (Radford, 2009).
(5)
(a) Mary, who has nine cats in her house, got married
yesterday.
(b) Peter got married to Mary, who has nine cats in her
house.
The adjective clauses in (5) who has nine cats in her
house are the DP post-modifier to modify the person. The
examples presented show that adjective clauses can be
placed in the medial position and final position. However,
they cannot be positioned in the initial position of the
sentence.
This point allows us to see that adjective clauses are
Afr Educ Res J 70
heavy-ended constructions. They are used to express
supplemental information, such as numerical data,
examples, classifications and details. However, adjective
clauses can be reduced into reduced adjective clauses,
such as (6).
(6)
(a) Mary, having nine cats in her house, got married
yesterday.
(b) Peter got married to Mary, having nine cats in her
house.
The present participles having in (6a) and (6b) show that
the adjective clauses were reduced.
Related previous studies
The literature review shows that the end-weight principle
was examined using different materials, such as English
novels and poetry, international corpora as in British
National Corpus (BNC) and EFL learners’ written texts.
This section presents the similarities and differences in
the results of the end-weight principle in different texts.
Novels and poetry in English
Mothibeli (2023) studied the different positions of that-
nominal clauses in English novels, as exemplified in (7).
(7)
(a) I forgot that I was wearing iron shoes.
(b) We will teach this miserable traitor that he cannot
undo our work so easily.
(Mothibeli, 2023, p. 37)
Although that-nominal clauses can be used in different
positions, such as the subject and the object, the majority
of the that-nominal clauses were usually found in the final
position. The that-nominal clause in (7a) is the direct
object of the sentence. It can be replaced by the pronoun
it as in I forgot it. On the other hand, the that-nominal
clause in (7b) is syntactically classified as an adjunct.
Removing this information does not impact the
grammaticality of the sentence. With this information,
Mothibeli (2023) explained that the final position of that-
nominal clauses was explained by the end-weight
principle. Although the structure of that-nominal clause
complements is applicable grammatically in the subject
position, such as (8), Mothibeli (2023) said that this
violates the principle of end-weight.
(8) That nothing of the kind existed in writing was
certainly true. (Mothibeli, 2023, p. 39)
Violating the end-weight principle does not mean that it is
wrong grammatically. However, there is another effective
way to structure example (8) as in it is certainly true that
nothing of the kind existed in writing. In addition, Nofal
(2012) found that the relative clauses in poems were
written in the final position, such as (9).
(9)
(a) I said to my soul, be still, and let the dark come upon
you which shall be the darkness of God.
(b) And smale foweles maken melody, that slepen at the
nyght with open eye.
(Nofal, 2012, p. 233)
According to Nofal (2012), not only do the relative
clauses in poems comply with the end-weight principle
but also the final position is kept for the most complex
part. In addition, Leech (2007) also found that the end-
weight principle is a common stylistic practice in English
fiction. Heavy constituents follow light constituents, as in
(10).
(10) Ours was the marsh country. (Leech, 2007, p. 122)
The syntactic structure in (10) is subjective complement
where the complement provides additional information
about the subject. The two positions between the subject
and complement could be swapped, while they denote
the same semantically. However, the longer part is placed
in the final position.
International corpora
Previous studies used international corpora to study the
end-weight principle. Park (2002) employed corpus tools
such as BNC and LOB to study the ordering of adverbial
clauses in English. The results show that the majority of
adverbial clauses are used in the final position to indicate
adverbial clauses of time, place and manner. These are
the additional information or details, which are considered
the new piece of information. This complies with the
pragmatic discourse of given and new information where
new information is structurally placed at the end.
Not only does heavier information go to the end of the
sentences, but also complex structures are placed at the
final position. Complex structures are exemplified into
adverbial clauses, adjective clauses, and noun clauses
and the head of the clauses are under the CP nodes
syntactically, such as (11).
(11)
(a) Add some salt if needed.
(b) James knows that this is a difficult job.
(c) GER 101 is an elective course, which is useful for
first-year language students.
Examples (11a) are adverbial clauses. The connector if is
Wongkittiporn 71
a concessive adverbial connector, which is located under
the branch of the complementizer phrase if. In (11b), that
this is a difficult job is a noun clause. The relativizer that
is located under the C node. In (11c), which is useful for
first-year language students is an adjective clause. It
provides the author’s subjectivity concerning the elective
course GER101.
(12)
(a) I gave several historical research papers to Mary.
(b) I gave Mary several historical research papers.
Ottmar (2015) indicated that the structure presented in
(12b) rather conforms to the end-weight principle and
sounds more English than (12a).
Arnold et al. (2000) studied heavy NP shift in the
corpus in the Canadian parliament. The data was
gathered from the spoken register. The two structures,
(V+NP+PP) and (V+PP+NP), were compared. The
results showed that the heavy NP was shifted to the final
position. The explanation for this shift is fixed colligation
as the VP and NP are formulaically used together, as in
bring to and take into account.
EFL learners writing
The end-weight principle is commonly investigated
through L2 writing. Sung and Kim (2016) investigated the
end-weight principle with non-native English learners.
They used the Yonsei English Learner Corpus (YELC)
which is the corpus of non-native English language
learners to investigate the end-weight principle via the
learners argumentative essays. 39 essays were collected
to be studied. The results of their study showed that most
advanced English learners follow the end-weight principle
in English, while lower proficiency users are likely to
violate the rule.
Lozano and Mayamendikoetxea (2009) found in their
experiment that L2 English language learners and native
English speakers can apply the end-weight principle in
their production process. This study is interesting as it
points out the number of words to be judged as it is a
long constituent.
[…] There is no agreement in the literature as to the
most appropriate measuring instrument for syntactic
weight. While word length, as measured by a number of
words, is standardly used, it cannot tell us how long (or
short) a constituent must be to be considered “heavy” (or
“light”). […] the most frequent lengths for the subject
constituent in the learner corpora were 2 and 5 words,
and the mean of all lengths was 7.5 words. (Lozano and
Mayamendikoetxea, 2009, p. 483)
A constituent that is interpreted to be heavy and long
contains between seven to eight words. Lozano and
Mayamendikoetxea (2009) found that the mean score of
length in the final position is 7.52, despite some
students creating a length of 13 to 19 words at the end.
(13)
(a) In 1880, it begun the experiments whose result was
the appearance of television some years later.
(b) Thus began the campaign to educate the public on
how one contracts aids.
(Lozano and Mayamendikoetxea, 2009, p. 490)
Although both L2 English language learners and native
speakers can apply the end-weight principle correctly, the
average length of heaviness in the final position was
reported to be different. The average range of the final
position as produced by L2 English learners is longer
than native English speakers. Okuyama (2015) found that
engineering academic research papers used by-phrase
agents with the long-expression for the information of
mathematic calculation. This complies with the end-
weight principle.
Applied linguistics articles
While previous studies used the materials of novels,
international corpora and L2 learners’ writing, this study
contributes something new to the field by using the
materials of applied linguistics research articles as
indexed in the SCOPUS database to test the end-weight
principle in the English language. Wongkittiporn (2023)
addressed the advantages of using English-applied
linguistics research papers to study. Firstly, the papers
are written by academic people in the field of language.
Some people have many years of experience in using
language in the field, so the materials can be used as
examples for learners. Secondly, the journal publication
teams have editors and proofreaders. Not only do the
editors screen the information in the articles to be
published, but there is a team of reviewers to give
feedback regarding the accuracy of the content. After
that, the copyediting process allows the proofreaders to
check the accuracy of the language used. Thirdly, most
graduate courses provide courses in research
methodology. However, the language used in writing
research is not yet taught officially in the class. After
studying the subject of research methodology, most
students can figure out which methods are appropriate for
their research papers. Nevertheless, they struggle to
express their ideas linguistically according to the norms
of research writing. This study provides examples for
novice researchers and new scholars who are just
entering the field to structure their research writing
accurately and appropriately.
METHOD
Unlike previous studies, the current study is a quantitative
Afr Educ Res J 72
study to test the hypothesis of the end-weight principle.
The experiments were divided into two phases. The first
phase is the experiment of the end-weight principle on
applied linguistics articles from the SCOPUS Q1
database. After that, the end-weight principle was tested
with Thai EFL learners descriptive essays.
Participants and materials
For the first experiment, the data collected in this study
was 20 academic research articles from the Q1 SCOPUS
database, consisting of approximately 200,000 words.
There are a total of 117 sentences containing adjective
clauses in the final position. The data were collected for
System and Journal of English for Specific Purposes from
2022 to 2024. These two journals were selected because
they are applied linguistics journals that have been
indexed in the Q1 SCOPUS database for longer than a
decade. This ensures the validity and reliability of these
two publications.
In the second experiment, the population in this study is
Thai high-school EFL learners. In Thailand, this group of
learners is called Mattayom learners. The participants in
this study were gathered by the convenience sampling
method. They willingly volunteered to join this writing
activity. They studied in either Thai private or public
schools under the supervision of the Ministry of
Education, Thailand. The sample consisted of 67
students. 45 students came from public schools, whereas
22 participants came from private schools. In the process
of recruiting the participants, a private university in
Thailand organized an academic writing contest where
the information about the event was posted on the
internet and the university website. The participants
register for the competition free of charge. It was a
national competition and the winner received a trophy
from the Ministry of Culture, Thailand. This ensured that
the participants put in optimal effort and paid close
attention when writing their essays. The essays were
written in a controlled room at a private university in
Thailand with five proctors to control the process of
writing.
Data collection
For the first experiment, in order to analyze syntactic
structures in academic research papers, the purposive
sampling method was adopted in this study. The inclusion
criteria of data collection in the study follow Okuyama
(2015).
Condition A: The journal is highly ranked in the field.
Condition B: The university affiliation is the United States
of America, the United Kingdom or Australia.
These criteria help refine the quality of the writers. Not
only are they native American, British or Australian
students, native lecturers, scholars or professors, but
non-native students who can enter American, British and
Australian universities are required to have the minimal
requirement of either TOEFL or IELTS scores. Moreover,
highly ranked journals usually have proofreaders to check
the accuracy of language before the paper is published.
For the second experiment, the data collection came
from Thai EFL learners writing. They were asked to write
a 250-word academic essay. The duration of writing was
120 minutes. The topic of writing was “Thai Soft Power”,
which is a campaign of the Thai government from 2023 to
2024. This topic is about descriptive writing and adjective
clauses are likely to be used often in this type of essay.
Approximately 17,000 words provided 36 sentences
containing adjective clauses. In the writing process, the
participants were asked to type their academic essays in
Microsoft Word. Therefore, the tokens of adjective
clauses were manually extracted and kept in Microsoft
Excel for subsequent study. The grammatical errors
found were kept as original.
Data analysis
In order to seek correlation as stated in the objectives of
the study, this study was quantitatively designed. The
theoretical concept of the end-weight principle originally
follows Quirk et al. (1972) as elaborated in the section of
the literature review. Adjective clauses in this study follow
Radford (2009) who classified the adjective clauses as
complementizer phrases (CP) structurally. The keys for
the examination in this CP are who, which, that, whom
and whose, such as (14).
(14)
(a) Xxxxxxxxxxxxxxxxxxxxxxx [CP which
ooooooooooooooooooooo].
(b) Xxxxxxxxxxxxxxxxxxxxxxx, [CP which
ooooooooooooooooooooo].
The objective of this study is to test the end-weight
principle only. As such, restrictive relative clauses and
non-restrictive relative clauses are not the main focus of
this study. Both were gathered to study. With these
patterns, the coding schema in this study is given in Table
1. (15)
After that, the code was put into SPSS29 in order to
calculate the significant relationship of Pearson
correlation. The p-value in this study is set up at 0.05.
The P-value of two tails was chosen. In this study, the
correlation was significant when the p-value was reported
as equal to or less than 0.05 (Rasinger, 2013). The value
of r is between 0 and 1 (Rasinger, 2013). If the r value is
closer to 1, it is interpreted to have a higher correlation.
Wongkittiporn 73
Table 1. Coding schema.
Condition A
Compliance with end-weight principle
Condition B
Non-compliance with end-weight principle
Code 1 when adjective clauses are placed at the final
position. This was interpreted as compliance with the end-
weight principle in English. Accordingly, code 1 was given.
Examples
Mary visited Schönbrunn Palace that is located in Vienna,
Austria.
Thailand is a tourist destination because of its own culture
which attract people all over the world.
Code 2 when reduced adjective clauses are placed at the
final position. This was interpreted as violating the end-
weight principle in English. Accordingly, code 2 was given.
Examples
Mary visited Schönbrunn Palace located in Vienna,
Austria.
The vehicle itself is more of a bike than a car, having
three wheels.
Tail 1
Tail 2
Figure 2. P-value of two tails.
The p-value of two tails is applied in this study (Figure 2).
This means that the significant relationship can be
reported with minus. With this methodology, the results of
the study are given in the following section.
RESULTS
Results of Experiment 1
From a total of approximately 200,000 words, there were
117 tokens of both adjective clauses and reduced
adjective clauses occurring in the final position in applied
linguistics research articles. Their statistical data is
reported in Table 2.
Table 2 reveals the statistically significant relationship
between the theory of end-weight principle and adjective
clauses in applied linguistics articles as indexed in
SCOPUS Q1 databases. The p-value was reported at
.001, which means that there was a significant correlation
between the two variables. In addition to a quantitative
study, this section presents the qualitative results of the
adjective clauses and reduced adjective clauses in
applied linguistics research articles.
Table 2. Correlation between end-weight principle and adjective clauses in applied linguistics articles.
Correlations
Adjective clauses Eng-weight principle
Tokens of adjective clauses in applied linguistics articles
Pearson Correlation 1 .297**
Sig. (2-tailed) .001
N 117 117
** Correlation is significant at the 0.01 level (2-tailed).
Although the majority of adjective clauses were used in
the final position, there were some cases where reduced
adjective clauses were used in the final position. Some
examples of adjective clauses and reduced adjective
clauses in applied linguistic articles were given as
follows:
(16)
(a) Additionally, the participants were confident about
their communicative competence (M = 3.83, SD = 0.68),
which indicated that they were confident to communicate
in Korean (M = 3.75, SD = 0.55). (Kim and Yuzhu, 2024,
p. 7)
Afr Educ Res J 74
(b) Building upon these studies, our research takes a
novel approach by adopting the perspective of
translanguaging as co-learning which encourages
participants to play fluid expert/learner roles during the
exchange to investigate translingual and transmodal
practices in teletandem from a social semiotics lens.
(Satar, Aranha, Cavalari and Almijiwl, 2024, p. 1)
(c) This aligns with an influential view in cognitive
psychology which states that auditory processing can be
considered domain-general and this ability forms the
basis of multiple domain-specific phenomena. (Ruan and
Saito, 2023, p. 1)
(d) The engagement rate of tweets is a common way to
measure interactions with social media posts; it is based
on a post’s “engagement actions,” which include replies,
retweets (when another user simply retweets the original
post), and likes. (Tardy, 2023, p. 14)
The adjective clause in (16a) provided numerical data
through a mean score standard deviation. The adjective
clause in (16b) gave extra information about the
advantages of co-learning. The adjective clause in (16c)
is the definition of cognitive psychology. The adjective
clause in (16d) provided the elements of engagement
action. Therefore, adjective clauses provide
supplementary data concerning numerical information,
definitions, and explanations of terms and components.
Despite having many examples of adjective clauses
concerning form and meaning, reduced adjective clauses
were also found in applied linguistics articles, such as
(17).
(17)
(a) On the other hand, turbulences in international
relations and the outbreak of a global pandemic have
increased uncertainties, posing threats and disruption to
people’s daily lives, leading to school closures, travel
restrictions and precarious working prospects. (Wu and
Forbes, 2023, p. 2)
(b) The results showed that the three subtests were
clustered as one single factor (explaining 75.5% of the
variances), suggesting that the three subtests may have
tapped into one single aspect of participants’ auditory
processing ability. (Ruan and Saito, 2023, p. 6)
The reduced adjective clause in (17a) denoted a
consequence of a certain action. The reduced adjective
clause in (17b) provided a recommendation of a certain
result. Reduced adjective clauses were for the semantic
denotations of consequences and suggestions.
However, after taking a closer look, the semantic
denotation of consequences was also found to be used
with adjective clauses, such as (18).
(18) Rather, the presented analysis focuses on the
development of one interdiscursive trajectory, which was
the most consequential in terms of the evolving
relationship between the researcher and participant.
(Shahri, 2023, p. 3)
With this evidence, semantic denotations were the
variable to manipulate the variance of adjective clauses
and reduced adjective clauses in the final position. If
English was an end-weight language where adjective
clauses are not normally reduced at the final position, it
would be interesting to establish why writers use the
reduced adjective clauses in the final position in applied
linguistic articles. Okuyama (2015) also found that
engineering academic research papers used by-phrase
agents with the long-expression for the information of
mathematic calculation. This complies with the end-
weight principle. The explanation was given in the
discussion section.
Results of Experiment 2
This experiment of two alternative variances between
adjective clauses and reduced adjective clauses
confirmed the theory that English is an end-weight
language. However, there was an exception in that the
reduced adjective clauses in English in the final position
could be used to avoid the horror-aequi principle. This
first experiment shows that those writers of the SCOPUS
Q1 database from well-known American, Australian and
British universities strictly follow the principle of end-
weight in their writing as it was an acceptable condition of
information structure in standard English and academic
writing.
Once the researcher linguistically understood the
theory of end-weight and its exceptions, the researcher
used this theory and exceptions to continue the
experiment on Thai EFL learners who studied in Thai high
schools, which are known as Mattayom. They were
neither international students nor international schools.
They were Thai Mattayom students who studied at either
public or private schools in Thailand. The objective of the
second experiment was to examine whether Thai
Mattayom students had this principle embedded in their
brains or not.
The results showed that Thai Mattayom EFL learners
followed the end-weight principle the same way as the
writers in SCOPUS Q1. The quantitative results of this
study are given in Table 3.
Table 3 reveals the inferential statistical results between
the end-weight principle and the adjective clauses in Thai
Mattayom EFL learners essays. The p-value was
reported at 0.02 and this means that there were
statistically significant relationships between the two
variables.
Although there were two variances between adjective
clauses and reduced adjective clauses to select for the
final position of the sentence, they knew that adjective
clauses should be filled in the final position. However,
Wongkittiporn 75
there was only one occurrence where an EFL writer used
a reduced adjective clause in the final position. Both
examples are given below.
(19)
(a) Thailand has one of the most beautiful temples in the
world. People often travel there by the iconic Tuk Tuk,
which is a three-wheel mini car with open spaces for
you to enjoy
(b) Some famous Thai dishes are Tom Yom and Som
Tom, which are found in almost every part of
Thailand.
(c) Thailand is a tourist destination because of its own
culture which attracts people from all over the world.
(d) The vehicle itself is more of a bike than a car, having
three wheels.
Table 3. Correlation between end-weight principle and adjective clauses in Thai EFL learners’ essays.
Correlations
Adjective clauses Eng-weight principle
Adjective clauses in Thai EFL learners’ essays
Pearson Correlation 1 .385*
Sig. (2-tailed) .020
N 36 36
*Correlation is significant at the 0.05 level (2-tailed).
Examples (19) showed the use of adjective clauses and
reduced adjective clauses by Thai EFL learners. In (19a),
the writer could use appositive NP while the relativizer
which and the auxiliary are could have been deleted.
However, the writer chose to use an adjective clause at
the final position. Similarly, another option for (19b) and
(19c) was the present participle and the past particle as in
found and attracting, respectively. However, only one
token was found to violate the horror-aequi principle as a
reduced adjective clause in (19d) was used without any
repetitive form in an adjacent area.
DISCUSSION
English as an end-weight language
The inferential statistical data in this study proves that
English is an end-weight language (Quirk et al, 1972).
Although there are two grammatical variances between
adjective clauses and reduced adjective clauses, the
writers selected the variance of adjective clauses, which
are longer, to place at the final position. However, there
are a few tokens that are contradictorily used with the
theory. This means that the authors used reduced
adjective clauses in the final position, such as (20).
(20)
(a) The results showed that the three subtests were
clustered as one single factor (explaining 75.5% of the
variances), suggesting that the three subtests may have
tapped into one single aspect of participants’ auditory
processing ability. (Ruan and Saito, 2023, p. 6)
(b) Moreover, […] reported that academic collocations
were learned significantly better than technical
collocations, suggesting that the type of collocations can
also be a factor in learning through listening. (Tuzcu,
2023, p. 4)
The writers used reduced adjective clauses together with
adjective clauses to avoid the repetition of the same form
in adjacent areas. This is technically known as horror
aequi principle (Ruohonen and Rudanko, 2019). The
repetition is modified in (21).
(21) Moreover, […] reported that academic collocations
were learned significantly better than technical
collocations which suggests that the type of
collocations can also be a factor in learning through
listening.
Writers are often fearful of repeating the relativizer that
twice in adjacent areas. Therefore, the use of the first that
relativizer is deleted, while the verb suggest is modified
into the present participle.
In addition to the end-weight principle, the topic or the
spec T position in English is normally short and
nominalized (Wongkittiporn, 2023). This is because the
subject is an old piece of information that has already
been elaborated on in the previous discourse. Once the
writers want to link the topic with the previous discourse,
they normally summarize it. For example, the
nominalization of the sentence the car is red becomes
the red car.
While most previous studies addressed this section
with the errors that EFL learners made in their writing,
this section addresses the causes of accuracy. The high
correlation between the end-weight principle and
adjective clauses in Thai EFL learners essays is not
accidental, but there are several reasons to explain
Afr Educ Res J 76
this phenomenon.
Avoidance of reduced adjective clauses
It is difficult for Thai EFL learners to produce reduced
adjective clauses. Wongkittiporn (2024) investigated Thai
EFL learners' use of reduced adjective clauses. The
results showed that they have difficulty in using reduced
adjective clauses at approximately 75 percent. To be able
to use reduced adjective clauses correctly, EFL learners
are required to have the notion of syntactic features of
deletion and inflectional morpheme (Radford, 2009) as
explained in (22).
(22)
(a) Thailand is a tourist destination because of its own
culture which attracts people all over the world.
(b) Thailand is a tourist destination because of its own
culture attracting people all over the world.
Example (22a) represents adjective clauses, whereas
example (22b) represents reduced adjective clauses. To
transform the adjective clause in (22a) into the reduced
adjective clause in (22b), the syntactic feature of wh-
deletion must be applied at the same time as auxiliary be.
The verb attract must be changed into a present participle
via the deviation of the inflectional morpheme -ing as in
attracting. Due to this complexity, the variance of reduced
adjective clauses tends to be avoided by Thai EFL
learners. This interpretation of EFL learners avoiding the
use of reduced adjective clauses is relevant as this essay
writing competition was a national competition. The
structures used in their writing were thought about
carefully as making mistakes led to a score deduction.
Therefore, they were likely to avoid using complex
structures with a lot of modification, which may have led
to errors.
Mother tongue influence
The Thai language is an end-weight language. Although
Thai is a null subject-parameter, where the subject of the
sentence can be omitted without ungrammaticality, the
sentences in Thai could be continued where the writers
provide a number of modifiers at the final position
(Timyam and Bergen, 2010). When the same principle
between the two languages is shared in common
between the first and the second language, it is likely to
reduce the errors in the production of the second
language. One prominent study to support this argument
was written by Leenakitti and Pongpairoj (2019).
(23)
(a) (…) Mr. H. the person who had ordered Allan
Mitchison’s execution.
ายเอช ผู ั่ ฆ อัลล มิตช
(Black and Blue as cited in Leenakitti & Pongpairoj,
2019, p. 122)
(b) (…) had one tooth which overlapped another(…)
(…) ฟนสองซี่ เกยกันอยู (…)
(Black and Blue as cited in Leenakitti and Pongpairoj,
2019, p. 122)
Although Leenakitti and Pongpairoj (2019) show the
different strategies of English-Thai translation in novels,
the end-weight principle is there, which has never been
changed.
CONCLUSION
This study examines the theory of end-weight principle in
English adjective clauses between the two different
samples of publication of writers in SCOPUS Q1
databases and Thai EFL learners in order to answer the
following questions. To answer research question one,
there is a statistically significant relationship between the
end-weight principle and English adjective clauses in
applied linguistics articles indexed in the SCOPUS Q1
database. The author selected the variance of adjective
clauses to be placed at the final position of the sentences
although there is another variance of reduced adjective
clauses, which can make the sentence shorter. The
results of this study seem to suggest that post-verbal or
post-VP position is where complex structures, such as
adjective clauses are placed. Even though a previous
study found that the end-weight principle is associated
with advanced language learners, this might be true in
some cases. However, this study suggests that the issue
of the similarity of information structure between the first
language and the English language is also relevant. The
English language and the Thai language are the same in
that they are both languages of end-weight. The results of
this study show that it is possible for the first language to
show positive interference with the second language,
where Thai high-school students could apply the
information structure of adjective clauses pragmatically,
the same as SCOPUS Q1 writers.
The results in this study theoretically contribute to the
end-weight principle, which is viewed as a significant
notion in writing English. Previous studies gave examples
of different texts relating to the end-weight principle in
English. However, this study uses an inferential statistical
approach to study this theory. This study is innovative
methodologically in that the results gained by this study
could provide both external validity and add more
examples to the field. With this method, it seems fair to
claim that English is a heavy-ended language.
Applying functional grammar or functionalism is
recommended when teaching complex sentences.
Complex structures, such as adjective clauses are
encouraged to be taught at the same time as the end-
Wongkittiporn 77
weight principle, or packaging structure. This allows
English language learners to have the conceptual
framework in their mind that a longer clause like an
adjective clause is usually placed at the final position.
(24) I like ice cream, which is creamy and custard-based.
Example (24) shows that the subject of the sentence is
DP and nominalization, which is short and subjective. On
the other hand, the adjective clause is placed at the final
position of the sentence as it is longer and more
informative. Teaching this way allows English language
learners to understand the concept that nominalization
should be placed at the spec T or the subject position, but
complex clauses are post-verbal structures. This helps to
answer why the adjective clauses in English cannot occur
in the initial position of the sentence as it will violate the
end-weight principle. However, the results of the end-
weight principle in this study can be generalized to only
applied linguistics research articles and Thai EFL
learners essays. Generalizing the data in this study to
other areas might not be applicable to optimal levels. In
other words, the data in this study were specific to only
two groups of the population, and the writing cannot be
generalized to other writing styles of Thai EFL learners. It
is recommended for future studies that the quantitative
study of reduced adjective clauses in the medial position
of sentences would contribute something new to the field.
As mentioned at the beginning of this study, this study
pedagogically and innovatively contributes to EFL
learners. English language teachers normally pay
attention to teaching grammar and vocabulary to their
students because they are important to support their
learners writing. This is true in some cases. However,
only a few English teachers teach students how to
combine clauses. Teaching this aspect could make your
EFL learners different from others.
REFERENCES
Aarts, B. (2011). Oxford Modern English Grammar. Oxford University
Press.
Arnold, J. E., Losongco, A., Wasow, T., and Ginstrom, R. (2000).
Heaviness vs. newness: The effects of structural complexity and
discourse status on constituent ordering. Language, 76(1), 28-55.
Birner, B. J., and Ward, G. (2009). Information structure and syntactic
structure. Language and Linguistics Compass, 3(4), 1167-1187.
Kim, A., and Yuzhu, S. (2024). How implementing an AI chatbot impacts
Korean as a foreign language learner’s willingness to communicate in
Korean. System, 122, 103256.
Leech, G. (2007). Style in fiction revisited: the beginning of great
expectations. Style, 41(2), 117-132.
Leenakitti, N., and Pongpairoj, N. (2019). A study of translation of
relative clauses from English into Thai. PASAA: Journal of Language
Teaching and Learning in Thailand, 58, 95-130.
Lozano, C., and Mendikoetxea, A. (2009). L2 syntax meets information
structure: Word order at the interfaces. Proceedings of the 33rd
annual Boston university conference on language development, 313-
324.
Mothibeli, K. P. (2023). The syntactic functions of wh- and that-
nominals clauses in English language with reference to animal farm
by George Orwell: The case of dialogue section. ISRG Journal of
Arts, Humanities and Social Sciences, I(VI), 35-40.
Nofal, K. H. (2012). Syntactic aspects of poetry: A pragmatic
perspective. Asian Social Science, 8(2), 221-238.
Nofal, K. H. (2013). Darkness in Conrad's heart of darkness: A linguistic
and stylistic analysis. The Buckingham Journal of Language and
Linguistics, 6(1), 77-93.
Okuyama, Y. (2015). Analysis of the use of passive voice in academic
articles in engineering. Language Education & Technology, 52(1),
345-358.
Ottmar, E. (2015). Watch your weight! A corpus-based study of end-
weight constructions in the speech of native speakers and Swedish
learners of English. Bachelor Thesis, Department of Languages and
Literature, University of Gothenburg, Sweden.
Park, T. S. (2002). Clause ordering in English complex sentences. SNU
Working Papers in English Language and Linguistics, 1(1), 92-108.
Pribylová, A. (2019). The end-weight principle and word order in
English. Bachelor Thesis, Faculty of Humanity, Tomas Bata University
in Zlin.
Quirk, R., Greenbaum, S., Leech, G., and Svartvik, J. (1972). A
grammar of contemporary English. Longman.
Radford, A. (2009). An introduction to English sentence structure.
Cambridge University Press.
Rasinger, S. M. (2013). Quantitative research in linguistics: An
introduction. A&C Black.
Ruan, Y., and Saito, K. (2023). Less precise auditory processing limits
instructed L2 speech learning: Communicative focus on phonetic
form revisited. System, 114, 103020.
Ruohonen, J., Rudanko, J. (2019). Comparing explanatory principles of
complement selection statistically: a case study based on Canadian
English, Studia Neophilologica, 91(3), 296-313.
Satar, M., Aranha, S., Cavalari, S. M. S., and Almijiwl, W. (2024). Low
proficiency level learners' translingual and transmodal practices in
teletandem: Challenging the separation of languages
principle. System, 120, 103187.
Shahri, N. N. (2023). A four-year trajectory of development from student
to published researcher: Learning to collaborate in the research
article genre. Journal of English for Research Publication
Purposes, 4(2), 164-188.
Smolka, V. (2017). What comes first, what comes next: Information
packaging in written and spoken language. Acta Universitatis
Carolinae Philologica, 1(1), 51-61.
Sung, M. C., and Kim, H. (2016). Tracing developmental changes in L2
learners' structuring of phrasal verbs: A corpus study of native and
non-native argumentative essays. 3L: Southeast Asian Journal of
English Language Studies, 22(2), 151-166.
Tardy, C. M. (2023). How epidemiologists exploit the emerging genres of
Twitter for public engagement. English for Specific Purposes, 70, 4-
16.
Timyam, N., and Bergen, B. K. (2010). A contrastive study of the
caused-motion and ditransitive construction in English and Thai:
Semantic and pragmatic constraints. H.C. Boas (eds.). In Contrastive
Studies in Construction Grammar (pp. 137–168). John Benjamins
Tuzcu, A. (2023). The effects of input mode in learning declarative and
non-declarative knowledge of L2 collocations. System, 113, 103006.
Wasow, T. (1997). End-weight from the speaker's perspective. Journal
of Psycholinguistic Research, 26(1), 347-361.
Wongkittiporn, A. (2023). Pragmatic perspectives of the raised DP in
raising and passive constructions in applied linguistics
articles. Liberal Arts Review, 18(1), 40-64.
Wongkittiporn, A. (2024). Thai EFL undergraduate students’ attitudes
towards syntactic structures in English writing. Journal of English
Language and Linguistics, 5(1), 1-16.
Wu, X., and Forbes, K. (2023). Contextualizing the temporal dimension
in multilingual identity construction: A longitudinal Q methodological
study with Chinese LOTE-learning public high school
students. System, 116, 103090.