9201*5(7<60&62=.9:2;@9201*5(7<60&62=.9:2;@
(&$,174*9:9,12=.(&$,174*9:9,12=.
*,<4;@"<+42,*;276:

$.?<24;79$*6,;2B,*;276%1.6-29.,;#74.7/#.42027:2;@76$.?<24;79$*6,;2B,*;276%1.6-29.,;#74.7/#.42027:2;@76
$.?<*4$*;2:/*,;276$.?<*4$*;2:/*,;276
*;1*6.761*9-;
&62=.9:2;@7/%7976;7
.*6<:+@
9201*5(7<60&62=.9:2;@"97=7
-.*6)+<:+@+@<.-<
92*6'2447<01+@
9201*5(7<60&62=.9:2;@"97=7
7447>;12:*6-*--2;276*4>793:*;1;;8::,174*9:*9,12=.+@<.-</*,8<+
"*9;7/;1.!;1.9$7,2*4*6-.1*=279*4$,2.6,.:75576:
!92026*4"<+42,*;2762;*;276!92026*4"<+42,*;2762;*;276
.761*9-; <:+@'2447<01+@$.?0<24;79:*6,;2B,*;276%1.26-29.,;
974.7/9.42027:2;@76:.?<*4:*;2:/*,;276":@,17470@7/#.420276*6-$8292;<*42;@
(&$,174*9:9,12=.2;*;276(&$,174*9:9,12=.2;*;276
.761*9-; *;1*6<:+@.*6*6-'2447<01+@92*6$.?<24;79$*6,;2B,*;276%1.6-29.,;
#74.7/#.42027:2;@76$.?<*4$*;2:/*,;276
*,<4;@"<+42,*;276:

1;;8::,174*9:*9,12=.+@<.-</*,8<+
%12:"..9#.=2.>.-9;2,4.2:+97<01;;7@7</79/9..*6-78.6*,,.::+@(&$,174*9:9,12=.;1*:+..6
*,,.8;.-/7926,4<:27626*,<4;@"<+42,*;276:+@*6*<;1792A.-*-5262:;9*;797/(&$,174*9:9,12=.79579.
26/795*;27684.*:.,76;*,;.44.6)*5*;*60.47+@<.-<
Sex Guilt or Sanctification? The Indirect Role of Religiosity on
Sexual Satisfaction
Nathan D. Leonhardt
University of Toronto
Dean M. Busby and Brian J. Willoughby
Brigham Young University
With a Mechanical Turk sample of 1,614 sexually active individuals (62.6% women, 85% heterosexual,
mean age of 34.47 years) who had been in a committed sexual relationship for at least two years, we used
structural equation modeling to better understand how global religiosity may indirectly influence sexual
satisfaction. Because religiosity has been linked to the way people make sense of sexuality, we assessed
positive (sexual sanctification) and negative (sexual guilt) meaning making variables as mediators
between religiosity and sexual satisfaction. Consistent with prior research, greater sanctification of
sexuality was directly tied to greater sexual satisfaction, whereas greater sexual guilt was directly tied to
less sexual satisfaction. Greater general religiosity was indirectly related to greater sexual satisfaction for
men and women through greater sexual sanctification. Contrary to expectations, no significant pathways
emerged between greater religiosity and less sexual satisfaction via sexual guilt, possibly due to reliance
on a one item indicator for the latter variable. Also, in structural equation models, when sanctification of
sexuality was taken into account, greater religiosity was directly tied to less sexual satisfaction for
women, but not for men. This suggests that sanctification of sexuality represents a facet of religiousness
that facilitates women’s and men’s sexual satisfaction, whereas other religious beliefs may inhibit
women’s sexual satisfaction.
Keywords: religiosity, sexual sanctification, sexual guilt, sexual satisfaction
Religion brings potential for joy, pain, healing, hurt, esteem,
shame, love, and hate (Dollahite, Marks, & Dalton, 2018). These
paradoxical dualities (i.e., opposites or contrasts) have been well
documented in general psychology of religion and spirituality
(Pargament, 1997) and specifically in couple relationships (Ma-
honey, 2010; Mahoney, Pargament, Tarakeshwar, & Swank,
2001). Notably, different facets of religiosity have been linked to
relationships of gender inequality (e.g., Bartkowski, 2001; Sigalow
& Fox, 2014) and spousal abuse (e.g., Simonic, Mandelj, &
Novsak, 2013; Stotland, 2000), while also being connected to
relationships with high satisfaction (e.g., Fincham, Beach, Lam-
bert, Stillman, & Braithwaite, 2008; Olson, Marshall, Goddard, &
Schramm, 2015) and commitment (e.g., Fincham & Beach, 2014;
Lambert & Dollahite, 2008). Sexuality is one aspect of a couple’s
relationship where some researchers have suggested the impor-
tance of considering dualities (Christopher & Sprecher, 2000;
Hernandez, Mahoney, & Pargament, 2014; Murray-Swank, Parga-
ment, & Mahoney, 2005), as scholars have found positive (Hardy
& Willoughby, 2017; Murray-Swank et al., 2005) and negative
associations (Davidson, Moore, & Ullstrup, 2004) as well as the
absence of an association (Ashdown, Hackathorn, & Clark, 2011;
Young, Denny, Luquis, & Young, 1998) between indicators of
religiosity and sexual satisfaction.
A dualistic perspective (a long-held approach recently high-
lighted in a synthesis; Dollahite et al., 2018) may clarify incon-
sistent findings between global religiosity (i.e., overall religious
devotion) and sexual satisfaction, as a number of mechanisms may
explain how religiosity could both negatively and positively influ-
ence sexual satisfaction. Recently, scholars have suggested that the
inconsistent link may be due to neglecting explanatory variables
between religiosity and sexual satisfaction. Rather than having a
direct link, religiosity may indirectly influence sexual satisfaction
by informing sexual meaning or attitudes (e.g., Hackathorn, Ash-
down, & Rife, 2016; Hardy & Willoughby, 2017). Consistent with
the suggestion of religious dualities, religiosity has been linked to
both positive and negative sexual meaning constructs. On the
negative, religiosity has been linked to overall heightened sexual
concerns and discomfort (Cowden & Bradshaw, 2007), particu-
larly to sexual guilt (i.e., a negative affective component, com-
prised of self-imposed punishment for either violating or expecting
to violate “proper” sexual conduct; Hackathorn et al., 2016; Woo,
Brotto, & Gorzalka, 2011). In turn, sexual guilt has been linked to
lower sexual satisfaction (Hackathorn et al., 2016; Higgins, Trus-
sell, Moore, & Davidson, 2010). Conversely, religiosity has also
been linked to a positive valuation of sexuality (Hardy & Wil-
loughby, 2017; Hernandez, Mahoney, & Pargament, 2011;
Hernandez-Kane & Mahoney, 2018), specifically to sexual sanc-
tification (i.e., believing sexuality to have divine character and
This article was published Online First January 7, 2019.
Nathan D. Leonhardt, Department of Psychology, University of Toronto;
Dean M. Busby and Brian J. Willoughby, School of Family Life, Brigham
Young University.
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Nathan
D. Leonhardt, Department of Psychology, University of Toronto, 3359
Mississauga Road, Toronto, ON L5L1C6, Canada. E-mail: nathan
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
Psychology of Religion and Spirituality
© 2019 American Psychological Association 2020, Vol. 12, No. 2, 213–222
1941-1022/20/$12.00 http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/rel0000245
213
significance; Hernandez et al., 2011). Sexual sanctification in turn
has been positively linked to sexual satisfaction (Hernandez et al.,
2011; Hernandez-Kane & Mahoney, 2018). We suggest that by
simultaneously considering how religiosity may indirectly influ-
ence sexual satisfaction through both a negative valuation (sexual
guilt) and a positive valuation (sexual sanctification), we can begin
to better understand previous conflicting findings.
In this study, using a sample of 1,614 adults in committed sexual
relationships, we explored the extent that religiosity indirectly
influences sexual satisfaction through sexual guilt and sexual
sanctification. By including both negative and positive valuations
of sexuality, we hoped to provide an initial inquiry into the
dualities between religiosity and sexual satisfaction with a large
scale, quantitative analysis.
Sexual Guilt
With some religious assertions of sexuality throughout history
being connected to original sin, a barrier to be overcome, an open
sore that must be scratched, and a natural depravity of the human
body (Boulder, 1998; Schnarch, 1991), it seems plausible that
some religious individuals could have discomfort and conflict
surrounding sexuality. Freud (1927/1961) was among the earliest
in the social sciences to note a precarious relationship between
religiosity and sexuality, commenting on how pessimistic and
inhibitory emphasis in religion has been used to regulate sexual
conduct, an idea that now has empirical validation (e.g., Buss,
2002). This negatively imbued, highly regulated outlook on sex-
uality appears to be one reason that religiosity has been consis-
tently connected to an overall uneasiness surrounding sexuality,
particularly sexual guilt (e.g., Emmers-Sommer, Allen, Schoen-
bauer, & Burrell, 2018; Murray, Ciarrocchi, & Murray-Swank,
2007; Woo, Morshedian, Brotto, & Gorzalka, 2012). Logically,
viewing sexuality as something inherently dirty, debased, or a
necessary evil for procreation could result in feelings of moral
disapprobation when participating in a sexual act. Little research
has directly analyzed the negative connection between sexual guilt
and sexual satisfaction (for exceptions, see Hackathorn et al.,
2016; Higgins et al., 2010). However, many other studies indi-
rectly suggest a negative connection, as a recent meta-analysis
found that sexual guilt was linked to lower sexual activity, less sex
information accuracy, and less positive attitudes toward sex
(Emmers-Sommer et al., 2018).
Religiosity appears to be connected to sexual guilt for unmarried
individuals (Hackathorn et al., 2016), as most religious teachings
are particularly restrictive of sexual behavior outside of a hetero-
sexual marriage (Delamater, 1981; Miracle, Miracle, & Baumeis-
ter, 2003). However, few research findings exist on religiosity and
sexual guilt for couples within a committed or married relation-
ship. Those that do exist present a somewhat inconsistent picture.
Considering that many religions fully sanction sexuality within a
marital relationship (Hernandez et al., 2014), some have suggested
that sexual guilt may be unlikely to exist for religious individuals
who channel their sexuality into a God sanctioned marriage (Dol-
lahite, Marks, & Goodman, 2004). However, some studies have
shown that sexual guilt continues to pervade religious, married
individuals (Peterson, 1964), as some scholars have suggested that
religiosity can bring a more permanent sense of anxiety, guilt, and
tension surrounding sexuality (Runkel, 1998). This is perhaps due
to some religious teachings condemning any sexual activity only
for pleasure, rather than for procreation (Abbott, Harris, & Mollen,
2016; Cowden & Bradshaw, 2007). Therefore, research suggests
that religiosity could negatively influence sexual satisfaction by
imbuing the sexual relationship with sexual guilt, though the role
of marital status remains a matter of inquiry for this connection.
Sexual Sanctification
Although some scholars have begun to question whether West-
ern culture’s religious roots have led to an exclusively negative
view of sexuality, others suggest religiosity has the potential to
also imbue sexuality with positive meaning (Hernandez et al.,
2014). Some Western religious teachings have sanctioned sexual-
ity not only for its procreative potential to be cocreators with God,
but also for bonding, pleasure, and enhancing a committed cou-
ple’s union with each other and God (Hernandez et al., 2014;
Lauer, 1985; Turner, Fox, & Kiser, 2007).
Sanctification could be one specific positive meaning making
process that could help religious individuals have a better sexual
relationship. The concept of sanctification has received ample
theoretical and empirical consideration for the sanctification of
marriage (e.g., Ellison, Henderson, Glenn, & Harkrider, 2011;
Kusner, Mahoney, Pargament, & Demaris, 2014) or a romantic
relationship (Fincham, Lambert, & Beach, 2010), parenting (e.g.,
Brelsford, 2013; Weyand, O’Laughlin, & Bennett, 2013), and even
the body (e.g., Homan & Boyatzis, 2009; Jacobson, Hall, &
Anderson, 2013). More specifically, sexual sanctification (Hernan-
dez et al., 2011; Hernandez-Kane & Mahoney, 2018; Murray-
Swank et al., 2005) explores how religious individuals may believe
in a spiritual dimension of sexuality, and how that perspective may
heighten affect during and after the sexual experience. It involves
a “setting apart” of sexuality from the ordinary, denoting a con-
secration to God. Regardless of debates concerning perception or
reality about the sacred, reporting sanctification is real in its
consequences; evidence suggests that reporting something to be
sacred influences the way someone treats the aspect of life that has
been “set apart” (for a more extensive review of studying the
sacred, see Pargament, Oman, Pomerleau, & Mahoney, 2017).
On a more specific empirical note, sexual sanctification has
been linked to higher sexual satisfaction, sexual intimacy, marital
satisfaction, and spiritual intimacy (Hernandez et al., 2011). Ad-
ditionally, greater sanctification of marital sexuality early in mar-
riage has predicted more frequent sexual intercourse, sexual sat-
isfaction, and marital satisfaction one year later (Hernandez-Kane
& Mahoney, 2018). Therefore, research suggests that religiosity
could positively influence sexual satisfaction by imbuing the sex-
ual relationship with a sanctified, divine sense of meaning.
Gender Considerations
An additional, understudied consideration is whether there
might be any differences in how religiosity influences sexual
meaning making and sexual satisfaction for women in comparison
to men. Some have asserted that Christian theology (a theology
belonging to a high percentage of our participants) emphasizes the
“dangers” of female sexuality, without giving the same attention to
male sexuality (Farley, 1976; Nicolson, 1993). Additionally, am-
ple research has shown differences between men and women in the
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
214
LEONHARDT, BUSBY, AND WILLOUGHBY
domain of sexuality (e.g., Leonhardt & Willoughby, 2017; Mc-
Clelland, 2010; Petersen & Hyde, 2010) and religiosity (e.g.,
Ahrold & Meston, 2010; Luquis, Brelsford, & Rojas-Guyler,
2012). On average, men tend to be more permissive than women
in their sexual attitudes (Petersen & Hyde, 2010) and women tend
to be more religious than men (e.g., Ahrold & Meston, 2010;
Luquis et al., 2012). More specific to our study, women are
significantly more likely than men to experience sexual guilt and
anxiety (Emmers-Sommer et al., 2018; Oliver & Hyde, 1993;
Petersen & Hyde, 2010). These differences in sexual attitudes
(particularly sexual guilt; Emmers-Sommer et al., 2018; Oliver &
Hyde, 1993), and religiosity (Ahrold & Meston, 2010; Luquis et
al., 2012), justify conducting separate analyses for men and
women.
Current Study
In line with a dualistic perspective, religiosity seems to have the
potential to lead to either sexual guilt or sexual sanctification. By
evaluating religiosity’s indirect pathways to sexual satisfaction
through both sexual guilt and sexual sanctification, with a national
sample of 1,614 adults in long-term, committed relationships, we
hoped to provide a first empirical look at religiosity’s potential for
duality in the sexual relationship. To help ensure our sample
consisted of committed individuals, we required participants to be
in a sexual relationship at least two years, as this is approximately
the period when partners become more securely attached to each
other and some of the early, obsessive romantic love often starts to
wane (Mikulincer & Shaver, 2016). Although commitment in a
relationship likely plays an important role for some couples in
whether someone feels sexual guilt or sexual sanctification, it is
important to note that most religious teachings are particularly
restrictive of sexual behavior outside of a heterosexual marriage
(DeLamater, 1981; Miracle et al., 2003). This could make marital
status an important control throughout our analyses. On the basis
of previous research highlighted in our review, we proposed the
following hypotheses and research questions (RQs):
H1: Sexual guilt will be negatively associated with sexual
satisfaction, whereas sexual sanctification will be positively
associated with sexual satisfaction.
H2: Religiosity will be positively associated with both sexual
guilt and sexual sanctification.
H3: Religiosity will have an indirect effect on sexual satis-
faction through sexual guilt and sexual sanctification.
RQ1: What are the similarities or differences for men and
women?
RQ2: What role does marital status play in these associations?
Method
Sample and Procedure
The sample of the present study consisted of 1,614 sexually
active individuals (606 men; 1,008 women) sampled from the
Amazon Mechanical Turk (MTurk) website (http://www.mturk
.com), who had been in a committed, romantic relationship for at
least two years and were residents of the United States. MTurk is
a website dedicated to online labor and is used to employ “work-
ers” from around the world to complete specific tasks. The study
was approved by the institutional review board. Regarding the
current project, a job was posted inviting participants to complete
a short survey on sexuality in a relationship. Participants were
instructed that they needed to be English speaking, and those who
were interested were directed to a separate website (Qualtrics) to
complete an online assessment. Before beginning the online sur-
vey, participants were asked to indicate informed consent and were
informed about their rights as a research participant. Upon com-
pletion of the survey, participants were thanked for their time and
given compensation of $0.50. Scholars have noted that samples
from MTurk are very similar to other diverse behavioral research
samples, and several scholars have replicated previous research
results on MTurk to provide evidence for the validity of the sample
(Paolacci, Chandler, & Ipeirotis, 2010; Suri & Watts, 2011). Full
demographic descriptive statistics are listed in Table 1.
Measures
Religiosity. Four items were used to assess global religiosity:
“Spirituality is an important part of my life” (1 never,5 very
often), “How often do you pray (commune with a higher power)?”
(1 never,5 very often), “How important is your religious faith
to you” (1 not important,5 very important), and “How often
do you attend religious services?” (1 never,5 weekly). The
Cronbach’s alpha was acceptable for both men (.92) and women
(.90).
1
Sexual guilt. Each individual answered the following ques-
tion, “I experience guilt or anxiety when it comes to my sex life.”
Responses ranged on a five-point Likert scale (1 strongly
disagree,5 strongly agree). The item came from the Sexual
History and Adjustment Questionnaire (Lewis & Janda, 1988).
Sexual sanctification. An adaptation of Hernandez et al.
(2011) sanctification of sexuality measure, specifically the 10-item
Sacred Qualities subscale, was utilized to assess sexual sanctifi-
cation in the relationship (e.g., “Our sexual relationship is holy,”
“The sexual bond I have with my partner is sacred to me”). The
only adaptation was using language inclusive of committed rela-
tionships broadly rather than referring explicitly to marriage (a
similar approach to Fincham et al., 2010). Responses ranged on a
seven-point Likert scale (1 strongly disagree,7 strongly
agree). The Cronbach’s alpha was .95 for both men and women.
Sexual satisfaction. The Global Measure of Sexual Satisfac-
tion (GMSEX; Lawrance & Byers, 1995) was created to assess
overall sexual satisfaction. The GMSEX has consistently been
1
Because our measure of global religiosity consisted of both subjective
valuation of religiosity as well as a report of behavior, we conducted
several supplementary analyses to be more confident in our findings. Our
SEM using religiosity without the behavior item was consistent with our
reported results. There were also no major changes to results if the
subjective valuation scale and behavior item were evaluated as separate
variables in the final SEM. The overall story was consistent of religiosity
connected to sexual satisfaction primarily through sexual sanctification.
Because of (a) the high factor loadings of each item onto global religiosity
and because (b) the difference of intrinsic versus extrinsic religiosity was
not a key component of our research question, we decided the most
appropriate course of action was model the four items as indicators of
global religiosity in the analyses. Full results are available on request.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
215
GUILT OR SANCTIFICATION
shown to have some of the strongest psychometric validity among
sexual satisfaction measures (Mark, Herbenick, Fortenberry, Sand-
ers, & Reece, 2014). The GMSEX has a root question of “In
general, how would you describe your sexual relationship with
your partner?” The participants then respond to five separate items,
each on a seven-point scale, in reporting their overall sexual
satisfaction: (1) good–bad, (2) pleasant–unpleasant, (3) positive–
negative, (4) satisfying–unsatisfying, and (5) valuable–worthless.
Our sample had high reliability for men (␣⫽.95) and women
(␣⫽.96).
Control variables. Age, relationship length, and number of
children were each assessed with open ended questions. Race,
sexual orientation, and marital status were dichotomized so that all
participants were coded as either White (1) or non-White (0),
heterosexual (1) or nonheterosexual (0), and married (1) or non-
married (0). Education was assessed on a seven-point scale (1
less than high school,7 doctorate; e.g., PhD, JD, MD), and
income was assessed on a 12-point scale (1 none,12
$300,000 or above). Marital status was a particularly important
control, as some scholars have suggested that sexual guilt from
religion may be unique to unmarried individuals (Hackathorn et
al., 2016).
2
Data Analysis Plan
We first conducted preliminary analyses to gain greater insight
into our variables of interest and explore whether we were justified
in splitting our analyses by gender. We conducted a multivariate
analysis of variance (MANOVA) by gender (see Table 2), bivari-
ate correlations (see Table 3), and measurement invariance tests.
After determining it was appropriate to split the sample by gender
(see further details in results section), we estimated a series of
structural equation models (SEM) each increasing in complexity.
For the MANOVA by gender and bivariate correlations, the items
for constructs were averaged together to create a mean score for
scales of interest. For both the measurement models and subse-
quent SEM models, all individual items of scales were modeled
onto latent variables for their corresponding constructs.
We first estimated measurement models to increase confidence in
the validity of these latent variables. This is an appropriate prelimi-
nary step detailed by Kline (2016) to help ensure that each item
adequately loads onto the appropriate corresponding latent construct
while all latent variables are taken into account simultaneously. This
increases confidence that each latent construct is a good fit for the data
before modeling additional observed variables and regression path-
ways that complicate the model. We then created SEM models for
men and women to estimate the direct effect of religiosity on sexual
satisfaction with all controls inserted. Age, race, education, income,
relationship length, sexual orientation, number of children, and mar-
ital status were used as controls in these models. Finally, we inserted
sexual sanctification, and sexual guilt as variables between religiosity
and sexual satisfaction and conducted 5,000 bootstraps to obtain
results for indirect effects (see Figure 1 for the hypothesized model).
All analyses were conducted in MPlus 7.11 (Muthén & Muthén,
1998 –2012).
Results
Preliminary Analyses
Gender MANOVA. Gender differences were explored for all
the variables. A MANOVA revealed an overall significant gender
difference: Wilks ␭⫽.866, F(26, 3204) 9.16, p .001, partial
eta-square .07. Levene’s test suggested that homogeneity of vari-
ance was violated for income (p .001), sexual orientation (p
.001), and sexual guilt (p .001). Accordingly, group comparisons
between men and women for variables where Levene’s test was
significant should be interpreted with caution. Table 2 shows means,
standard deviations, and F values for each variable.
2
In addition to our primary analyses where we controlled for marital
status, we conducted supplementary analyses to assess whether all findings
were consistent across unmarried and married individuals. For this partic-
ular sample there were no differences in findings for our constructs of
interest for those that were married as opposed to those who were unmar-
ried (results available on request).
Table 1
Descriptive Statistics for Demographic Variables
Variable M or % SD Range
Age 34.47 9.36 18–80
Relationship length 98.81 89.29 24–660
Gender
Men 37.4%
Women 62.6%
Sexual orientation
Heterosexual 85.0%
Sexual minority 15.0%
Race
White 77.9%
Asian 4.7%
African 6.5%
Latino 6.2%
Mixed/biracial 3.6%
Native American .6%
Other .6%
Relationship status
Married 55.6%
Cohabiting 27.1%
Dating, never married 13.4%
Other 3.9%
Education
High school or less 10.0%
Some college 41.7%
4-year degree 33.4%
Advanced 14.8%
Income
None 6.5%
Under $20,000 18.2%
$20,000–39,999 25.7%
$40,000–59,999 22.6%
$60,000–79,999 13.2%
$80,000 13.6%
Religious affiliation
Christian 48.7%
Atheist 9.1%
Agnostic 13.2%
No affiliation 19.5%
Other 9.5%
Number of children
0 44.6%
1 19.8%
2 21.0%
3 14.6%
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
216
LEONHARDT, BUSBY, AND WILLOUGHBY
Bivariate associations. We then assessed the bivariate asso-
ciations between the variables of interest for each gender. For
parsimony, we do not report here all the significant correlations
(see Table 3 for full results), but we note some of the most
important findings. As expected, religiosity was associated with
higher sexual sanctification (men: r .50, p .001; women: r
.34, p .001). However, religiosity was not related to sexual guilt
(men: r .03, p .45; women: r .04, p .18), or sexual
satisfaction (men: r .07, p .08; women: r ⫽⫺.01, p .79).
However, sexual sanctification was positively associated with sex-
ual satisfaction (men: r .37, p .001; women: r .45, p
.001) and sexual guilt was negatively associated with sexual sat-
isfaction (men: r ⫽⫺.39, p .001; women: r ⫽⫺.35, p .001).
Also of interest, sexual guilt and sexual sanctification only had a
small (men: r ⫽⫺.11, p .01) to moderate (women: r ⫽⫺.20,
p .001) negative correlation.
Measurement invariance. We found that the sexual sanctifi-
cation measure failed to meet the assumption for strong invariance:
2
difference(10) 44.896, p .001, suggesting that the con
-
struct does not meet the necessary conditions for direct comparison
between men and women (Dyer, 2015; Little, 2013). Considering
the (a) violation of Levene’s test for sexual guilt, (b) lack of
measurement invariance for sexual sanctification, and (c) previous
empirical and conceptual work suggesting differences between
men and women in the domain of sexuality (Emmers-Sommer et
al., 2018; Leonhardt & Willoughby, 2017; McClelland, 2010;
Petersen & Hyde, 2010), we decided that it would be more appro-
priate to estimate separate models for each gender rather than
making direct comparisons through a multigroup model.
Structural Equation Models
Measurement models. Both final measurement models fit the
data adequately (men:
2
[143] 580.44, CFI .96, p .001,
RMSEA .07, SRMR .06; women:
2
[138] 841.30, p
.001, CFI .97, RMSEA .07, SRMR .06). All factor
loadings were above .47 for both men and women.
Nonindirect effects structural equation models. The models
had good fit (men:
2
[82] 194.25, p .01 CFI .98,
RMSEA .05, SRMR .03; women:
2
[82] 254.87, p .001,
CFI .98, RMSEA .05, SRMR .02). For men, religiosity
was positively associated with sexual satisfaction (␤⫽.13, p
Table 2
Descriptive Statistics and Gender Differences of Control, Independent, and Dependent Variables
Variable
Men Women
Response rangeM SD M SD F
Age 36.17 10.91 33.47 10.34 18–80 24.69
ⴱⴱⴱ
White .79 .41 .78 .42 0–1 .21
Education 4.40 1.38 4.06 1.25 1–7 25.74
ⴱⴱⴱ
Income 4.33 2.02 3.42 1.60 1–12 100.47
ⴱⴱⴱ
Relationship length (months) 102.31 89.45 96.86 89.48 24–660 1.4
Number of children 1.06 1.29 1.23 1.89 1–7 3.94
Heterosexual .90 .31 .83 .38 0–1 14.22
ⴱⴱⴱ
Married .57 .50 .55 .50 0–1 .51
Religiosity 2.72 1.30 2.84 1.25 1–5 3.45
Sexual guilt 1.95 1.19 2.19 1.34 1–5 12.63
ⴱⴱⴱ
Sexual sanctification 4.01 1.67 4.01 1.67 1–7 .01
Sexual satisfaction 5.68 1.40 5.76 1.40 1–7 1.23
p .05.
ⴱⴱⴱ
p .001.
Table 3
Bivariate Correlations Between Control Variables, Independent Variables, and Outcome Variables
Variable 123456789101112
1. Age .11
ⴱⴱⴱ
.06 .20
ⴱⴱⴱ
.58
ⴱⴱⴱ
.27
ⴱⴱⴱ
.14
ⴱⴱⴱ
.22
ⴱⴱⴱ
.22
ⴱⴱⴱ
.09
ⴱⴱ
.02 .14
ⴱⴱⴱ
2. White .15
ⴱⴱⴱ
.04 .04 .11
ⴱⴱⴱ
.03 .09
ⴱⴱ
.00 .05 .03
3. Education .18
ⴱⴱⴱ
.05 .24
ⴱⴱⴱ
.01 .09
.03 .07
.00 .02 .11
ⴱⴱⴱ
.04
4. Income .23
ⴱⴱⴱ
.05 .38
ⴱⴱⴱ
.13
ⴱⴱⴱ
.02 .06 .15
ⴱⴱⴱ
.07
.04 .05 .01
5. Relationship length .59
ⴱⴱⴱ
.13
ⴱⴱ
.15
ⴱⴱⴱ
.22
ⴱⴱⴱ
.22
ⴱⴱⴱ
.11
ⴱⴱⴱ
.43
ⴱⴱⴱ
.17
ⴱⴱⴱ
.02 .01 .08
ⴱⴱ
6. Number of children .40
ⴱⴱⴱ
.07 .05 .21
ⴱⴱⴱ
.43
ⴱⴱⴱ
.11
ⴱⴱⴱ
.17
ⴱⴱⴱ
.21
ⴱⴱ
.03 .05 .06
7. Heterosexual .09
.04 .11
ⴱⴱ
.14
ⴱⴱⴱ
.09
.25
ⴱⴱⴱ
.05 .06
.03
8. Married .37
ⴱⴱⴱ
.23
ⴱⴱⴱ
.33
ⴱⴱⴱ
.50
ⴱⴱⴱ
.41
ⴱⴱⴱ
.15
ⴱⴱⴱ
.06 .00 .05
9. Religiosity .22
ⴱⴱⴱ
.13
ⴱⴱ
.13
ⴱⴱ
.15
ⴱⴱⴱ
.19
ⴱⴱⴱ
.28
ⴱⴱⴱ
.12
ⴱⴱ
.21
ⴱⴱⴱ
.04 .34
ⴱⴱⴱ
.01
10. Sexual guilt .05 .01 .04 .06 .02 .04 .09
.01 .03 .20
ⴱⴱⴱ
.35
ⴱⴱⴱ
11. Sexual sanctification .06 .12
ⴱⴱ
.01 .05 .05 .14
ⴱⴱⴱ
.12
ⴱⴱ
.09
.49
ⴱⴱⴱ
.11
ⴱⴱ
.45
ⴱⴱⴱ
12. Sexual satisfaction .16
ⴱⴱⴱ
.10
.05 .02 .14
ⴱⴱⴱ
.11
ⴱⴱ
.04 .14
ⴱⴱ
.07 .39
ⴱⴱⴱ
.37
ⴱⴱⴱ
Note. Men’s correlations are below the diagonal. Women’s correlations are above the diagonal. Correlations between two dichotomous variables are
precluded.
p .05.
ⴱⴱ
p .01.
ⴱⴱⴱ
p .001.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
217
GUILT OR SANCTIFICATION
.01). Of the control variables, age was negatively associated with
sexual satisfaction (␤⫽⫺12, p .02). For women, religiosity
was not associated with sexual satisfaction (␤⫽.05, p .20). Of
the control variables, age (␤⫽⫺14, p .01), was negatively
associated with sexual satisfaction. The models predicted a small
amount of the variance of sexual satisfaction for both men (R
2
.06) and women (R
2
.03).
Indirect effects model for men. The model had adequate fit
(
2
[287] 814.45, p .001 CFI .96, RMSEA .06, SRMR
.05), and predicted small variance for sexual guilt (R
2
.02), and
moderate variance for sexual sanctification (R
2
.31) and sexual
satisfaction (R
2
.29). Sexual sanctification was positively associ
-
ated with sexual satisfaction (␤⫽.35, p .001), and sexual guilt was
negatively associated with sexual satisfaction (␤⫽⫺.35, p .001).
Model 1a in Table 4 lists all other direct effects.
Religiosity had a significant total effect on sexual satisfaction (␤⫽
.13, p .03, 95% CI [.06, .21]), and a significant indirect effect on
sexual satisfaction (␤⫽.18, p .001, 95% CI [.12, .24]). Specifi-
cally, it had a positive indirect effect through sexual sanctification
(␤⫽.19, p .001, 95% CI [.14, .25]). See Table 5 for full results of
indirect effects.
Indirect effects model for women. The model had adequate
fit,
2
(287) 1390.05, p .001 CFI .95, RMSEA .06,
SRMR .05, and predicted small variance for sexual guilt (R
2
.02)
and sexual sanctification (R
2
.17), and moderate variance for sexual
satisfaction (R
2
.29). Sexual sanctification was positively associ
-
ated with sexual satisfaction (␤⫽.40, p .001), and sexual guilt was
negatively associated with sexual satisfaction (␤⫽⫺.29, p .001).
Model 1b in Table 4 lists all other direct effects.
Interestingly, religiosity did not have a significant total effect on
sexual satisfaction (␤⫽.05, p .19, 95% CI [–.01, .10]), but it did
have a positive indirect effect on sexual satisfaction (␤⫽.17, p
.001, 95% CI [.10, .18]). Specifically, it had a positive indirect effect
through sexual sanctification (␤⫽.16, p .001, 95% CI [.13, .19]),
and a negative direct effect (␤⫽⫺.10, p .01, 95% CI [–.15, .04])
independent of any mediator. See Table 5 for full results of indirect
effects.
Discussion
Consistent with previous research, our first hypothesis (H1) was
supported, as sexual guilt was negatively associated with sexual
-.105*
Sexual Guilt
Sexual
Satisfaction
Religiosity
Figure 1. Hypothesized model of indirect effects. Model will control for age, race, education, income, sexual
orientation, number of children, relationship length, and marital status.
Table 4
Model of Direct Effects Predicting Sexual sanctification, Sexual guilt, and Sexual Satisfaction
Variable
Model 1a Model 1b
Men () Women ()
Sexual
Sanctification
Sexual
Guilt
Sexual
Satisfaction
Sexual
Sanctification
Sexual
Guilt
Sexual
Satisfaction
Age .05 .06 .08 .10
.12
ⴱⴱ
.14
ⴱⴱⴱ
White .05 .00 .03 .01 .01 .06
Education .03 .07 .00 .12
ⴱⴱⴱ
.03 .01
Income .00 .10
.02 .09
ⴱⴱ
.05 .00
Relationship length .05 .01 .02 .02 .02 .01
Number of children .04 .03 .08 .02 .02 .04
Heterosexual .06 .09 .00 .01 .05 .03
Married .02 .01 .09
.02 .08 .02
Religiosity .54
ⴱⴱⴱ
.03 .05 .40
ⴱⴱⴱ
.07 .10
ⴱⴱ
Sexual sanctification .35
ⴱⴱⴱ
.40
ⴱⴱⴱ
Sexual guilt .35
ⴱⴱⴱ
.29
ⴱⴱⴱ
R
2
.31 .02 .29 .17 .02 .29
Note. Model 1a:
2
(287) 814.45, p .001, CFI .96, RMSEA .06, SRMR .05; Model 1b:
2
(287)
1390.05, p .001, CFI .95, RMSEA .06, SRMR .05.
p .05.
ⴱⴱ
p .01.
ⴱⴱⴱ
p .001.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
218
LEONHARDT, BUSBY, AND WILLOUGHBY
satisfaction (Hackathorn et al., 2016; Higgins et al., 2010), and
sexual sanctification was positively associated with sexual satis-
faction (Hernandez et al., 2011; Hernandez-Kane & Mahoney,
2018). This follows the logic of previous research, as the uncom-
fortable, inhibiting feelings of sexual guilt are likely to be con-
nected to greater difficulty enjoying a sexual experience. Alterna-
tively, the peace, transcendence, and divine meaning making of
sexual sanctification should be connected to more satisfying sexual
experience. Perhaps the most unique insight our study offers into
these findings is the clear distinction between and unique associ-
ations of sexual guilt and sexual sanctification. The two constructs
only had a small negative correlation between each other, and
independently had a moderate association with sexual satisfaction
in the full structural model. Perhaps future work in sexuality would
benefit from the simultaneous evaluation of positive and negative
sexual attitudes and meaning making with more in-depth mea-
sures.
Our second hypothesis (H2), that religiosity would be positively
associated with both sexual guilt and sexual sanctification, was
partially supported. Although religiosity was consistently associ-
ated with sexual sanctification for both men and women in both the
bivariate correlations and the structural equation model, the same
did not hold true for sexual guilt. Religiosity was not related to
sexual guilt for either gender in the bivariate correlations or
structural equation models. Holistically, these findings are encour-
aging for religious individuals, as they suggest the possibility of
religiosity instilling a positive message about sexuality (e.g., Her-
nandez et al., 2014; Lauer, 1985).
Despite no association between religiosity and sexual guilt in
this particular study, this does not suggest that researchers should
stop searching for better understanding of the connection between
religiosity and sexual guilt. It is worth noting that the link between
religiosity and sexual guilt seems more likely to exist in unmarried
college samples (Hackathorn et al., 2016). Considering we con-
trolled for relationship status and length, and they both had such
little predictive power in our final structural equation model,
perhaps the association between religiosity and sexual guilt is only
found when sexuality is occurring in a casual or uncommitted
relationship. In general, future research should consider potential
moderators between religiosity and sexual meaning making con-
structs, as there could be several factors that increase or decrease
the likelihood of religiosity shaping someone’s views of sexual
guilt or sexual sanctification.
Our third hypothesis (H3), that religiosity would have an indi-
rect effect on sexual satisfaction through sexual guilt and sexual
sanctification was partially supported, with some interesting gen-
dered nuances. The results for men were in line with our hypoth-
esis. In the model without sexual sanctification or sexual guilt,
men’s religiosity had a small, positive association with sexual
satisfaction. However, when both sexual guilt and sexual sancti-
fication were added to the model, the direct effect disappeared.
Interestingly, there was no indirect effect through sexual guilt,
suggesting that the association between religiosity and sexual
satisfaction only exists insofar as religiosity leads to sexual sanc-
tification. We interpret this result with caution, as multiple waves
of longitudinal data are needed to truly understand indirect effects.
However, for men, it appears that religiosity is only positively
related to sexual satisfaction insofar that men utilize their religious
experience to develop a sanctified view of sexuality. These results
support previous suggestions by scholars about the importance of
considering potential explanatory variables between religiosity and
sexual satisfaction to help clarify the associations (Hackathorn et
al., 2016; Hardy & Willoughby, 2017).
The results for women were more nuanced and perhaps provide
some of the clearest evidence for dualities pointed out in previous
research (Dollahite et al., 2018; Mahoney, 2010; Mahoney et al.,
2001). Interestingly, no association was found between religiosity
and sexual satisfaction in the model without sexual guilt or sexual
sanctification, but the indirect effects explored after adding sexual
guilt and sexual sanctification provide a complicated picture. Re-
ligiosity had a small, positive, indirect effect on sexual satisfaction
through sexual sanctification. Yet, with all these variables ac-
counted for simultaneously, religiosity also had a small negative
direct effect on sexual satisfaction, resulting in no association for
the total effect. This finding is somewhat similar to a previous
study showing that greater initial frequency of prayer significantly
predicted less sexual satisfaction longitudinally when simultane-
ously accounting for sanctification (Hernandez-Kane et al., 2018).
Overall, the association between religiosity and sexual satisfaction
for women is dualistic and complex. Although the negative asso-
ciation between religiosity and sexual satisfaction was not ac-
counted for with the sexual guilt item, religiosity seemed to be
connected to a conflicting influence on women’s sexual satisfac-
tion when controlling for sexual sanctification. The negative direct
effect from religiosity to sexual satisfaction after accounting for
sexual sanctification suggests there may be religious factors we
were unable to account for that negatively influence women’s
sexual satisfaction (Farley, 1976; Nicolson, 1993; Woo et al.,
2012).
Table 5
Model of Total, Indirect, and Direct Effects Predicting Sexual Sanctification, Sexual Guilt, and Sexual Satisfaction
Direct, Indirect, and Total Effects
Model 1a Model 1b
Men Women
SE CI SE CI
Religiosity¡Sexual Satisfaction (direct effect) .05 .05 [.14, .04] .10
ⴱⴱ
.04 [.15, .04]
Religiosity¡Sexual Sanctification¡Sexual Satisfaction .19
ⴱⴱⴱ
.03 [.14, .25] .16
ⴱⴱⴱ
.02 [.13, .19]
Religiosity¡Sexual Guilt¡Sexual Satisfaction .01 .02 [.04, 02] .02 .01 [.04, .00]
Religiosity¡Sexual Satisfaction (total indirect) .18
ⴱⴱⴱ
.04 [.12, 24] .14
ⴱⴱⴱ
.02 [.10, .18]
Religiosity¡Sexual Satisfaction (total effect) .13
ⴱⴱ
.05 [.06, .21] .05 .04 [.01, .10]
ⴱⴱ
p .01.
ⴱⴱⴱ
p .001.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
219
GUILT OR SANCTIFICATION
Future research should consider potential explanatory variables
for this negative association for women. Perhaps inhibition sur-
rounding sexuality could be a unique mediator between religiosity
and sexual satisfaction, stemming from a message de-emphasizing
sexual expression (Freud, 1927/1961), sometimes more specifi-
cally gendered toward women (Farley, 1976; Nicolson, 1993). Or
perhaps religious women receive less comprehensive sexual edu-
cation, resulting in less knowledge of sexual anatomy and func-
tioning, leading to a less fulfilling sexual relationship. The com-
plicated nature of female sexual anatomy likely makes knowledge
of sexual functioning more crucial for women than men in having
a fulfilling sexual experience. Amid these speculations, it is im-
portant to keep in mind that this negative direct effect was only
found after accounting for sexual sanctification.
Limitations and Future Directions
Although our study had several strengths, such as advancing a
theory on religious dualities (Dollahite et al., 2018; Mahoney,
2010; Mahoney et al., 2001) a large national sample, and advanced
methodology for complex hypotheses, the study did have a number
of limitations. One limitation was the use of Mechanical Turk as a
sampling frame. Although we went through a lengthy process to
ensure the responses to the questionnaire were high in quality,
researchers should attempt to replicate these findings in a more
representative sample.
A second limitation is the cross-sectional nature of the study, as
it precludes causal or directional evidence. We are confident in the
theoretical reasoning suggesting that religiosity can influence at-
titudes and meaning surrounding sexuality (e.g., Emmers-Sommer
et al., 2018; Woo et al., 2012), which in turn can influence sexual
satisfaction (e.g., Hackathorn et al., 2016; Hardy & Willoughby,
2017; Hernandez et al., 2011). However, the possibility remains
for inverse associations. Perhaps feelings of sexual guilt could
result in someone turning to religiosity as an act of recompense.
Perhaps a sanctified experience with sexuality might lead someone
to search out religiosity to make better sense of their transcen-
dence. Some time periods that might be interesting to evaluate
longitudinally include the transition from unmarried to married, or
from childlessness to parenthood, as these adjustments may bring
dramatic life changes that alter one’s views of the relationship, as
well as sexuality within the relationship.
Limitations also existed with measurement. Sexual sanctifica-
tion was assessed with only a nontheistic Sacred Qualities of
Sexuality subscale, rather than as part of the whole sanctification
measure that includes the Manifestation of God subscale (Hernan-
dez et al., 2011). Furthermore, the subscale was revised to be more
inclusive of nonmarried individuals, making it difficult to assess
the extent these results are directly comparable to other studies
(e.g., Hernandez et al., 2011; Hernandez-Kane & Mahoney, 2018)
on sexual sanctification, specifically in marital relationships. How-
ever, this approach has been utilized before by broadening a
marital sanctification measure to a relationship sanctification mea-
sure (Fincham et al., 2010). Perhaps additional studies can further
explore how some of these subscales might play out differently in
marriage versus other committed relationships. It is interesting to
note that many committed couples, who are not married, view their
sexual unions through a sacred lens, despite organized religions
teaching that sacred sex should be reserved for marriage (Hernan-
dez et al., 2014). Perhaps a better understanding of how sanctifi-
cation plays out differently across married individuals and highly
committed unmarried individuals could be nuanced by the mean-
ing individuals attach to marriage itself (Willoughby & James,
2017).
Sexual guilt was assessed with one item, leaving us unable to
fully account for the reliability and validity of the construct (De-
Vellis, 1991). An additional concern with the item is that it asked
about whether someone experienced sexual guilt or anxiety. This
ambiguity in measurement is certainly a limitation, as guilt and
anxiety are not necessarily the same thing. Future researchers
should use a full scale for the construct to ensure the validity of
these results. It would be beneficial for scholars to further inves-
tigate this matter with a more sophisticated measurement of sexual
guilt (perhaps the Moser Sex-Guilt Scale; Janda & Bazemore,
2011) separate from sexual anxiety to tease out these nuances with
greater depth and clarity. Overall, these measurement limitations,
particularly the one item for sexual guilt, preclude clear conclu-
sions, and mark a beginning point for investigating the dualities of
religiosity.
Other interesting future directions might be to assess how this
positive and negative meaning making surrounding sexuality
might exist simultaneously. For example, someone may experi-
ence a unique conflict if she or he received mixed messages about
the positive and negative nature of sexuality from religious leaders
(e.g., sexuality is a necessary evil for the good of procreation).
More fully exploring the influence of both positive and negative
effects may help to fully understand the recent theoretical asser-
tions on religious dualities (Dollahite et al., 2018; Hernandez et al.,
2014; Mahoney, 2010). It would also be beneficial to assess other
dimensions of sexuality, such as sexual frequency and sexual
intimacy. Although this study included sexual orientation as a
control, it could be interesting to delve more deeply into sexual
orientation differences on this topic, as some religions prohibit
nonheterosexual sexual relations (Hernandez et al., 2014). It would
also be beneficial to more fully consider the multifaceted nature of
religiosity, evaluating beliefs, practices, and communities, as well
as assessing whether any differences are found amid different
religious affiliations (e.g., Dollahite et al., 2004; Hardy & Wil-
loughby, 2017).
Conclusion
We have highlighted the complication and duality between
religiosity and sexuality. Although the study provides some en-
couraging results for religious individuals by showing religiosity’s
potential to positively influence sexual satisfaction through sexual
sanctification (particularly for men), the encouraging finding is
blunted with the inconsistent positive and negative connection
between religiosity and sexual satisfaction for women. Researchers
and clinicians should be wary of religiosity’s potential for harm in
the sexual domain. Religious teachings can include a negative,
sometimes even intensely negative messages about sexuality (e.g.,
Boulder, 1998; Schnarch, 1991) that can be guilt inducing (e.g.,
Emmers-Sommer et al., 2018; Hackathorn et al., 2016) and in
extreme cases even crippling to a sexual relationship (Ley, 2017).
However, all should be equally aware of religion’s potential for
good in the sexual domain, as a sanctified sexual experience can be
a conduit for transcendence, bliss, peace, and even healing for
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
220
LEONHARDT, BUSBY, AND WILLOUGHBY
committed couples (Hernandez et al., 2011; Kleinplatz et al.,
2009).
References
Abbott, D. M., Harris, J. E., & Mollen, D. (2016). The impact of religious
commitment on women’s sexual self-esteem. Sexuality & Culture, 20,
1063–1082. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s12119-016-9374-x
Ahrold, T. K., & Meston, C. M. (2010). Ethnic differences in sexual
attitudes of U.S. college students: Gender, acculturation, and religiosity
factors. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 39, 190–202. http://dx.doi.org/10
.1007/s10508-008-9406-1
Ashdown, B. K., Hackathorn, J., & Clark, E. M. (2011). In and out of the
bedroom: Sexual satisfaction in the marital relationship. Journal of
Integrated Social Sciences, 2, 40–57.
Bartkowski, J. (2001). Remaking the godly marriage. New Brunswick, NJ:
Rutgers University Press.
Boulder, M. (Trans). (1998). Confessions: St. Augustine. New York, NY:
Vintage Books.
Brelsford, G. M. (2013). Sanctification and spiritual disclosure in parent-
child relationships: Implications for family relationship quality. Journal
of Family Psychology, 27, 639 649. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a00
33424
Buss, D. M. (2002). Sex, marriage, and religion: What adaptive problems
do religious phenomena solve? Psychological Inquiry, 13, 201–203.
Christopher, F. S., & Sprecher, S. (2000). Sexuality in marriage, dating,
and other relationships: A decade review. Journal of Marriage and the
Family, 62, 999 –1017. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1741-3737.2000
.00999.x
Cowden, C. R., & Bradshaw, S. D. (2007). Religiosity and sexual concerns.
International Journal of Sexual Health, 19, 15–24. http://dx.doi.org/10
.1300/J514v19n01_03
Davidson, J. K., Moore, N. B., & Ullstrup, K. M. (2004). Religiosity and
sexual responsibility: Relationships of choice. American Journal of
Health Behavior, 28, 335–346. http://dx.doi.org/10.5993/AJHB.28.4.5
Delamater, J. (1981). The social control of sexuality. Annual Review of
Sociology, 7, 263–290. http://dx.doi.org/10.1146/annurev.so.07.080181
.001403
DeVellis, R. F. (1991). Scale development: Theory and applications.
Newbury Park, CA: SAGE.
Dollahite, D. C., Marks, L. D., & Dalton, H. (2018). How religion helps
and harms families: A conceptual model of a system of dualities at the
nexus of faith and family life. Journal of Family Theory & Review, 10,
219 –241. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/jftr.12242
Dollahite, D. C., Marks, L. D., & Goodman, M. (2004). Families and
religious beliefs, practices, and communities: Linkages in a diverse and
dynamic cultural context. In M. J. Coleman & L. H. Ganong (Eds.), The
handbook of contemporary families: Considering the past contemplating
the future (pp. 411– 431). Thousand Oaks, CA: SAGE. http://dx.doi.org/
10.4135/9781412976022.n24
Dyer, W. J. (2015). The vital role of measurement equivalence in family
research. Journal of Family Theory & Review, 7, 415– 431. http://dx.doi
.org/10.1111/jftr.12115
Ellison, C. G., Henderson, A. K., Glenn, N. D., & Harkrider, K. E. (2011).
Sanctification, stress, and marital quality. Family Relations, 60, 404
420. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1741-3729.2011.00658.x
Emmers-Sommer, T. M., Allen, M., Schoenbauer, K. V., & Burrell, N.
(2018). Implications of sex guilt: A meta-analysis. Marriage & Family
Review, 54, 417– 437. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/01494929.2017
.1359815
Farley, M. A. (1976). Sources of sexual inequality in the history of
Christian thought. The Journal of Religion, 56, 162–176. http://dx.doi
.org/10.1086/486481
Fincham, F. D., & Beach, S. R. H. (2014). I say a little prayer for you:
Praying for partner increases commitment in romantic relationships.
Journal of Family Psychology, 28, 587–593. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/
a0034999
Fincham, F. D., Beach, S. R. H., Lambert, N., Stillman, T., & Braithwaite,
S. (2008). Spiritual behaviors and relationship satisfaction: A critical
analysis of the role of prayer. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology,
27, 362–388. http://dx.doi.org/10.1521/jscp.2008.27.4.362
Fincham, F. D., Lambert, N. M., & Beach, S. R. H. (2010). Faith and
unfaithfulness: Can praying for your partner reduce infidelity? Journal
of Personality and Social Psychology, 99, 649 659. http://dx.doi.org/
10.1037/a0019628
Freud, S. (1961). The future of an illusion. New York, NY: Norton.
(Original work published 1927)
Hackathorn, J. M., Ashdown, B. K., & Rife, S. C. (2016). The sacred bed:
Sex guilt mediates religiosity and satisfaction for unmarried people.
Sexuality & Culture: An Interdisciplinary Quarterly, 20, 153–172.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s12119-015-9315-0
Hardy, S. A., & Willoughby, B. J. (2017). Religiosity and chastity among
single young adults and married adults. Psychology of Religion and
Spirituality, 9, 285–295. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/rel0000112
Hernandez, K. M., Mahoney, A., & Pargament, K. I. (2011). Sanctification
of sexuality: Implications for newlyweds’ marital and sexual quality.
Journal of Family Psychology, 25, 775–780. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/
a0025103
Hernandez, K. M., Mahoney, A., & Pargament, K. I. (2014). Sexuality and
religion. In D. L. Tolman & L. M. Diamond (Eds.), APA handbook of
psychology and sexuality: Vol. 2: Contextual approaches (pp. 425– 447).
Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. http://dx.doi
.org/10.1037/14194-013
Hernandez-Kane, K. M., & Mahoney, A. (2018). Sex through a sacred lens:
Longitudinal effects of sanctification of marital sexuality. Journal of
Family Psychology, 32, 425– 434. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/
fam0000392
Higgins, J. A., Trussell, J., Moore, N. B., & Davidson, J. K. (2010).
Virginity lost, satisfaction gained? Physiological and psychological sex-
ual satisfaction at heterosexual debut. Journal of Sex Research, 47,
384 –394. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/00224491003774792
Homan, K. J., & Boyatzis, C. J. (2009). Body image in older adults: Links
with religion and gender. Journal of Adult Development, 16, 230 –238.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10804-009-9069-8
Jacobson, H. L., Hall, M. E. L., & Anderson, T. L. (2013). Theology and
the body: Sanctification and bodily experiences. Psychology of Religion
and Spirituality, 5, 41–50. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0028042
Janda, L. H., & Bazemore, S. D. (2011). The Revised Mosher Sex-Guilt
Scale: Its psychometric properties and a proposed ten-item version.
Journal of Sex Research, 48, 392–396. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/
00224499.2010.482216
Kleinplatz, P. J., Ménard, A. D., Paquet, M. P., Paradis, N., Campbell, M.,
Zuccarino, D., & Mehak, L. (2009). The components of optimal sexu-
ality: A portrait of “great sex”. The Canadian Journal of Human Sexu-
ality, 18(1/2), 1–13.
Kline, R. B. (2016). Principles and practice of structural equation mod-
eling. New York, NY: Guilford Press.
Kusner, K. G., Mahoney, A., Pargament, K. I., & DeMaris, A. (2014).
Sanctification of marriage and spiritual intimacy predicting observed
marital interactions across the transition to parenthood. Journal of Fam-
ily Psychology, 28, 604 614. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0036989
Lambert, N. M., & Dollahite, D. C. (2008). The threefold cord: Marital
commitment in religious couples. Journal of Family Issues, 29, 592–
614. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0192513X07308395
Lauer, E. F. (1985). The holiness of marriage: Some new perspectives from
recent sacramental theology. Studies in Formative Spirituality, 6, 215–
226.
Lawrance, K., & Byers, E. S. (1995). Sexual satisfaction in long-term
heterosexual relationships: The interpersonal exchange model of sexual
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
221
GUILT OR SANCTIFICATION
satisfaction. Personal Relationships, 2, 267–285. http://dx.doi.org/10
.1111/j.1475-6811.1995.tb00092.x
Leonhardt, N. D., & Willoughby, B. J. (2017). Pornography, provocative
sexual media, and their differing associations with multiple aspects of
sexual satisfaction. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships. Ad-
vance online publication. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0265407517739162
Lewis, R. J., & Janda, L. H. (1988). The relationship between adult sexual
adjustment and childhood experiences regarding exposure to nudity,
sleeping in the parental bed, and parental attitudes toward sexuality.
Archives of Sexual Behavior, 17, 349 –362. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/
BF01541812
Ley, D. J. (2017, August). Overcoming religious sexual shame. Psychology
Today. Retrieved from https://www.psychologytoday.com/blog/women-
who-stray/201708/overcoming-religious-sexual-shame
Little, T. D. (2013). Longitudinal structural equation modeling. New York,
NY: Guilford Press.
Luquis, R. R., Brelsford, G. M., & Rojas-Guyler, L. (2012). Religiosity,
spirituality, sexual attitudes, and sexual behaviors among college stu-
dents. Journal of Religion and Health, 51, 601–614. http://dx.doi.org/
10.1007/s10943-011-9527-z
Mahoney, A. (2010). Religion in families 1999 to 2009: A relational
spirituality framework. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 72, 805–
827. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1741-3737.2010.00732.x
Mahoney, A., Pargament, K. I., Tarakeshwar, N., & Swank, A. B. (2001).
Religion in the home in the 1980s and 1990s: A meta-analytic review
and conceptual analysis of links between religion, marriage, and parent-
ing. Journal of Family Psychology, 15, 559 –596. http://dx.doi.org/10
.1037/0893-3200.15.4.559
Mark, K. P. K., Herbenick, D., Fortenberry, J. D., Sanders, S., & Reece, M.
(2014). A psychometric comparison of three scales and a single-item
measure to assess sexual satisfaction. Journal of Sex Research, 51,
159 –169. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/00224499.2013.816261
McClelland, S. I. (2010). Intimate justice: A critical analysis of sexual
satisfaction. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 4, 663–680.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1751-9004.2010.00293.x
Mikulincer, M., & Shaver, P. R. (2016). Attachment in adulthood, 2nd ed.:
Structure, dynamics, and change. New York, NY: Guilford Press.
Miracle, T. S., Miracle, A. W., & Baumeister, R. F. (2003). Human
sexuality: Meeting your basic needs. Upper Saddle River, NJ: Prentice
Hall.
Murray, K. M., Ciarrocchi, J. W., & Murray-Swank, N. A. (2007). Spiri-
tuality, religiosity, shame and guilt as predictors of sexual attitudes and
experiences. Journal of Psychology and Theology, 35, 222–234. http://
dx.doi.org/10.1177/009164710703500305
Murray-Swank, N. A., Pargament, K. I., & Mahoney, A. (2005). At the
crossroads of sexuality and spirituality: The sanctification of sex by
college students. The International Journal for the Psychology of Reli-
gion, 15, 199–219. http://dx.doi.org/10.1207/s15327582ijpr1503_2
Muthén, L. K., & Muthén, B. O. (1998–2012). MPlus user’s guide (7th
ed.). Los Angeles, CA: Author.
Nicolson, P. (1993). Deconstructing sexology: Understanding the patholo-
gisation of female sexuality. Journal of Reproductive and Infant Psy-
chology, 11, 191–201. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/02646839308403218
Oliver, M. B., & Hyde, J. S. (1993). Gender differences in sexuality: A
meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 114, 29–51. http://dx.doi.org/10
.1037/0033-2909.114.1.29
Olson, J. R., Marshall, J. P., Goddard, H. W., & Schramm, D. G. (2015).
Shared religious beliefs, prayer, and forgiveness as predictors of marital
satisfaction. Family Relations, 64, 519 –533. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/
fare.12129
Paolacci, G., Chandler, J., & Ipeirotis, P. G. (2010). Running experiments
on Amazon Mechanical Turk. Judgment and Decision Making, 5,
411–
419.
Pargament, K. I. (1997). The psychology of religion and coping: Theory,
research, practice. New York, NY: Guilford Press.
Pargament, K. I., Oman, D., Pomerleau, J., & Mahoney, A. (2017). Some
contributions of a psychological approach to the study of the sacred.
Religion, 47, 718 –744. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/0048721X.2017
.1333205
Petersen, J. L., & Hyde, J. S. (2010). A meta-analytic review of research on
gender differences in sexuality, 1993–2007. Psychological Bulletin, 136,
21–38. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0017504
Peterson, J. A. (1964). Education for marriage (2nd ed.). New York, NY:
Scribner.
Runkel, G. (1998). Sexual morality of Christianity. Journal of Sex &
Marital Therapy, 24, 103–122. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/00926239
808404924
Schnarch, D. M. (1991). Constructing the sexual crucible: An integration
of sexual and marital therapy. New York, NY: W. W. Norton Company.
Sigalow, E., & Fox, N. S. (2014). Perpetuating stereotypes: A study of
gender, family, and religious life in Jewish children’s books. Journal for
the Scientific Study of Religion, 53, 416 431. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/
jssr.12112
Simonic, B., Mandelj, T. R., & Novsak, R. (2013). Religious-related abuse
in the family. Journal of Family Violence, 28, 339 –349. http://dx.doi
.org/10.1007/s10896-013-9508-y
Stotland, N. L. (2000). Tug-of-war: Domestic abuse and the misuse of
religion. The American Journal of Psychiatry, 157, 696 –702. http://dx
.doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp.157.5.696
Suri, S., & Watts, D. J. (2011). Cooperation and contagion in web-based,
networked public goods experiments. PLoS ONE, 8356(3), e16836.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0016836
Turner, T. E., Fox, N. J., & Kiser, J. D. (2007). Uniting spirituality and
sexual counseling: Semitic traditions. The Family Journal, 15, 294 –297.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1066480707301615
Weyand, C., O’Laughlin, L., & Bennett, P. (2013). Dimensions of reli-
giousness that influence parenting. Psychology of Religion and Spiritu-
ality, 5, 182–191. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0030627
Willoughby, B. J., & James, S. L. (2017). The marriage paradox: Why
emerging adults love marriage yet push it aside. New York, NY: Oxford
University Press. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/acprof:oso/9780190296650
.001.0001
Woo, J. S. T., Brotto, L. A., & Gorzalka, B. B. (2011). The role of sex guilt
in the relationship between culture and women’s sexual desire. Archives
of Sexual Behavior, 40, 385–394. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10508-010-
9609-0
Woo, J. S. T., Morshedian, N., Brotto, L. A., & Gorzalka, B. B. (2012). Sex
guilt mediates the relationship between religiosity and sexual desire in
East Asian and Euro-Canadian college-aged women. Archives of Sexual
Behavior, 41, 1485–1495. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10508-012-9918-6
Young, M., Denny, G., Luquis, R. R., & Young, T. (1998). Correlates of
sexual satisfaction in marriage. The Canadian Journal of Human Sexu-
ality, 7, 115–127.
Received February 21, 2018
Revision received October 23, 2018
Accepted November 8, 2018
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
222
LEONHARDT, BUSBY, AND WILLOUGHBY