University of North Dakota University of North Dakota
UND Scholarly Commons UND Scholarly Commons
Theses and Dissertations Theses, Dissertations, and Senior Projects
January 2021
An Exploration Of Sexual Consent, Sexual Non-Consent, And An Exploration Of Sexual Consent, Sexual Non-Consent, And
Nonverbal Sexual Consent Communication Behaviors, Amongst Nonverbal Sexual Consent Communication Behaviors, Amongst
Community Stakeholders. Community Stakeholders.
Brent Stewart
How does access to this work bene;t you? Let us know!
Follow this and additional works at: https://commons.und.edu/theses
Recommended Citation Recommended Citation
Stewart, Brent, "An Exploration Of Sexual Consent, Sexual Non-Consent, And Nonverbal Sexual Consent
Communication Behaviors, Amongst Community Stakeholders." (2021).
Theses and Dissertations
. 4104.
https://commons.und.edu/theses/4104
This Dissertation is brought to you for free and open access by the Theses, Dissertations, and Senior Projects at
UND Scholarly Commons. It has been accepted for inclusion in Theses and Dissertations by an authorized
administrator of UND Scholarly Commons. For more information, please contact und.commons@library.und.edu.
An Exploration of Sexual Consent, Sexual Non-Consent, and Nonverbal Sexual Consent
Communication Behaviors, amongst Community Stakeholders.
by
Brent Stewart
Bachelor of Arts, University of Washington, 2011
Master of Arts, University of North Dakota, 2018
A Dissertation
Submitted to the Graduate Faculty
of the
University of North Dakota
in partial fulfillment of the requirements
for the degree of
Doctor of Philosophy
Grand Forks, North Dakota
August, 2021
ii
Name: Brent A Stewart
This document, submitted in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the
degree from the University of North Dakota, has been read by the Faculty Advisory
Committee under whom the work has been done and is hereby approved.
__________________________
__________
Kara Wettersten Ph.D.
__________________________
__________
Tamba-Kuii Bailey Ph.D.
__________________________
__________
Rae Ann Anderson Ph.D.
__________________________
__________
Cheryl Hunter Ph.D.
This document is being submitted by the appointed advisory committee as
having met all the requirements of the School of Graduate Studies at the University of
North Dakota and is hereby approved.
____________________________________
Chris Nelson
Dean of the School of Graduate Studies
7/26/2021
iii
PERMISSION
Title An Exploration of Sexual Consent, Sexual Non-Consent, and Nonverbal Sexual
Consent Communication Behaviors, amongst Community Stakeholders.
Department Counseling Psychology and Community Services
Degree Doctor of Philosophy
In presenting this dissertation in partial fulfillment of the requirement for a
graduate degree from the University of North Dakota, I agree the library of this University shall
make it freely available for inspection. I further agree that permission for extensive copying or
scholarly purposes may be granted by the professor who supervised my dissertation work or, in
her absence, by the Chairperson of the department or the Dean of the School of Graduate
Studies. It is understood that any copying or publication or other use of this dissertation or part
thereof for financial gain shall not be allowed without my written permission. It is also
understood that due recognition shall be given to me and to the University of North Dakota in
any scholarly use which may be made of any material in my dissertation.
Brent Stewart
May 3
rd
, 2021
iv
Table of Contents
INTRODUCTION .............................................................................................................. 9
Script Theory and Sexual Consent ................................................................................ 12
Sexual Consent.............................................................................................................. 14
Sexual Non-Consent ..................................................................................................... 17
Consent and Violence Prevention ................................................................................. 18
Nonverbal Consent........................................................................................................ 20
Consent Practices in Underrepresented Communities .................................................. 23
Delphi ............................................................................................................................ 25
Purpose of the Study ..................................................................................................... 26
METHODS ....................................................................................................................... 28
Participants .................................................................................................................... 28
Procedure ...................................................................................................................... 33
Data Analysis ................................................................................................................ 36
Data Analysis Round One ............................................................................................. 37
Data Analysis Round Two ............................................................................................ 37
Data Analysis Round Three .......................................................................................... 38
RESULTS ......................................................................................................................... 40
Sexual Consent Results ................................................................................................. 40
Sexual Non-Consent Results......................................................................................... 44
v
Nonverbal Sexual Consent Communication Behaviors Results ................................... 66
DISCUSSION ................................................................................................................... 77
Sexual Consent.............................................................................................................. 77
Sexual Non-Consent ..................................................................................................... 82
Nonverbal Sexual Consent Communication Behaviors ................................................ 85
Strengths and Limitations ............................................................................................. 88
Implications for Violence Prevention and Practice ...................................................... 89
Conclusion .................................................................................................................... 91
References ......................................................................................................................... 93
vi
List of Tables
Table 1 Delphi Rounds 1,2,3, Panelist Demographics
........................................................................................................................................... 30
Table 2 Round 2, Level of Agreement to Descriptions of Consent 1(This does not adequately
describe consent) to 5 (This very much describes consent) with Mean, Standard Deviation, and
Variance
.......................................................................................................................................... 43
Table 3 Round 2, Task 1, Qualitative Feedback to participants descriptions of Sexual Consent
........................................................................................................................................... 17
Table 4 Round 2, Sexual Consent Qualities Rank Order Position
........................................................................................................................................... 33
Table 5 Round 3, Sexual Consent Qualities Rank Order Position
........................................................................................................................................... 36
Table 6 Sexual Consent Qualities First Rank Order Position Comparison between Round Two
Ranking and Round Three Ranking,
........................................................................................................................................... 39
Table 7 Sexual Consent Qualities Sixth Rank Order Position Comparison between Round Two
Ranking and Round Three Ranking,
vii
........................................................................................................................................... 42
Table 8 Round 3, Task 1, Level of Agreement to Descriptions of Sexual non -consent 1(Strongly
Disagree) to 5 (Strongly Agree) with Mean, Standard Deviation, and Variance ............ 48
Table 9 Round 3, Task 1 Qualitative Feedback to participants descriptions
.......................................................................................................................................... 56
Table 10 Elements of Sexual Non-Consent Rank Order Position Round 3
........................................................................................................................................... 64
Table 11 Round One, Qualitative descriptions of missing nonverbal sexual consent
communication behaviors.
........................................................................................................................................... 67
Table 12 Round Three, Nonverbal Sexual Consent Communication Behavior Usage
........................................................................................................................................... 70
viii
ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS
This study is a result of a labor of love and is a testament to the support and unwavering faith of
the community which surrounded me as its’ author during the height of the dual pandemics of
COVID-19 and Racism. I would like to take a moment to acknowledge my parents (Brent and
Monica Stewart) for their faith, my grandmother (Margaret Hill) for her optimism, and my
siblings (Paula, Desmond, and Monique) for their cheers during this process. Additionally, I
would like to thank my midwestern family (Candice Farnham and the Motors) and my chosen
family in Seattle Washington (Peter Englund, Oliver Jones, Lauren Menor, Adam Panebianco,
Sam McQueen, Kate Gruver, Max Gray, Larissa Miller, Dr. Caroline Titan, Andrea Cerna, Jay
Miller). Next, I would like to acknowledge the deep support from my adviser (Dr. Kara
Wettersten), the UND faculty (including Drs. Tamba-Kuii Bailey; RaeAnn Anderson, Thomas
Motl, Ashley Hutchison and Cheryl Hunter) and the UND Housing Department (especially
Stuart Lickteig) all of whom helped make my dream of becoming a psychologist a reality.
Lastly, I would like to thank my colleagues Drs. Marisa Mango, Megan K. Smith, Natalie
Remanier, and Lauren Chapple-Love with whom I have the honor of sharing this journey with
and also share the privilege of calling UND their alma mater.
ix
ABSTRACT
Sexual Consent is a central concept in the field sexual violence and sexual violence
prevention (Beres, 2007). However, despite disproportional rates of sexual violence amongst
LGBT+ community, currently our understanding sexual consent and its practice is primarily
focused on heterosexual encounters of traditional college aged students (CDC, 2017,
Muehlenhard, Humphreys, Jozkowski & Peterson, 2016). The current study utilized the Delphi
method to develop a better understanding of sexual consent, sexual non-consent, and nonverbal
sexual consent communication behaviors among two distinct groups: sexual researchers and men
who have sex with men (MSM). Thirty-five panelists (13 researchers 22 MSM) completed one-
three rounds of an interactive study in which they provided 31 initial descriptions of sexual
consent and 20 descriptions of sexual non-consent. Through grounded theory analysis, these
descriptions were collapsed into 6 qualities of sexual consent and 5 elements of sexual non-
consent and ranked for importance. Panelists reviewed, critiqued, and sorted Beres et al. (2007)’s
list of nonverbal sexual consent communication behaviors. Implications of the perception of
these behaviors and implications for future research and practice are discussed.
ix
INTRODUCTION
In 2011, the nation focused its attention on sexual assault. Specifically, the Title IX Act
drew attention to sexual assault on college campuses through (Ali, 2011). The origins of title IX
stem from looking at equity amongst genders in terms of sports, but the Obama administration
was broadened to look at issues of identity-based harassment and sexual assault. This formal
adoption of sexual violence prevention as a governmental priority has provided a platform for
activists and advocates to bring sexual violence prevention to the forefront of the lives of many
Americans. This platform has included on campus trainings, documentary films regarding sexual
violence on college campuses, and most recently the #metoo movement (Airey, 2018;
Muehlenhard, Humphreys, Jozkowski, & Peterson, 2016).
Researchers of sexual violence have defined sexual violence broadly as “sexual contact
achieved without consent” (Beres, 2007; Halley, 2016, p. 262). Thus, the definition of sexual
violence depends heavily on the definition and conceptualization of sexual consent and how it is
communicated. Unfortunately, there is limited research on what constitutes sexual consent, and
an overall lack of consensus on what exactly sexual consent is or how it is communicated
between parties (Beres, 2007; Beres, 2014; Muehlenhard et al., 2016; Pugh & Becker, 2018).
Furthermore, when examining sexual consent, current research on consent and its
communications practices are centered around the experiences of white, heterosexual, cisgender,
individuals with varying levels of experience in sexual interactions (Beres et al., 2004;
Jozkowski, et al., 2014; Humphreys & Brousseau, 2010; Humphreys & Herold, 2007; Ward et
10
al., 2012). Thus, when considering sexual consent and its practice, and its relation to sexual
violence prevention it is paramount we broaden the scope of our understanding. This is
especially relevant given the documented disparities regarding the experience of sexual violence
within sexual minority and gender minority communities such as men who have sex with men
(MSM; Kosciw et al., 2016; Smith et al., 2018).
Further complicating this picture is the way we conceptualize the communication of
sexual consent between parties. Amongst sexual researchers, sexual consent communication has
been depicted with two clear sets of behaviors utilized by partners to convey and seek sexual
consent; verbal and nonverbal behaviors (Beres et al. 2004; Jozkowski et al., 2014). Verbal
behaviors largely fall within the realm of verbal communication and can include direct
communication, indirect communication, and “dirty talk” (Beres, et al. 2004; Hall, 1998;
Hickman & Muehlenhard, 1999; Jozkowski, et al. 2014). Broadly, nonverbal behaviors have
been labeled as “body language” and is considered to include behaviors such as hugging, kissing,
massaging, undressing, eyeing, self-stimulation of genitals, stimulation of partners’ genitals, and
non-resistance to sexual advances (Beres et al., 2004; Camilleri, et al., 2007). While both verbal
and nonverbal sexual consent communication behaviors are depicted across multiple studies,
current literature has depicted a clear preference amongst subjects for using nonverbal behaviors
rather than utilizing verbal communication behaviors (Beres et al., 2004; Hall, 1998; Humphreys,
2007; Jozkowski, et al., 2013 King, et al. 2020). Given this preference, it is important to consider
the impact these nonverbal communication behaviors have on the expression of sexual consent
especially within models of violence prevention such as affirmative consent.
11
Thus, it is scope of the current project to examine sexual consent communication
behaviors, specifically to examine the use of nonverbal sexual consent communication behaviors
amongst underrepresented populations in current literature. Utilizing the consensus-oriented
Delphi research methodology (Linstone & Turoff, 1975), the current study seeks to compare the
perceptions of sexual consent and sexual consent communication behaviors between sexual
researchers of any gender or sexual identity (SR) and men who have sex with men community
members (MSM). Specifically, the current study seeks to better understand how these two
groups conceptualize sexual consent, sexual non-consent, and nonverbal communication
behaviors of sexual consent.
Lastly, as part of this work, it is important to acknowledge the role that sexual consent,
most importantly the lack of sexual consent, plays in sexual violence. Within the scope of the
current study, it is the aim of the researcher to better understand nonverbal sexual consent
communication behaviors and their utilization within the process of sexual consent
communication. The current study is not designed to definitively define sexual consent, sexual
non-consent, or advocate for the replacement of verbal sexual consent communication behaviors
with nonverbal sexual consent communication behaviors. Rather, the current study seeks to build
a fuller picture of the sexual consent communication process specifically the use and
understanding of nonverbal sexual consent communication behaviors. Furthermore, because of
its intimate connections with sexual violence, it is also paramount to note the current study of
sexual consent communication behaviors is not designed to account for mal intent to cause harm.
Rather, this study seeks to create a fuller picture of sexual consent communication practices and
12
aide in the creation of even more effective sexual violence interventions and consent
communication practices that reflect the diversity of practices currently in use.
Script Theory and Sexual Consent
When discussing the concept of sexual consent and sexual consent communication
behaviors, it is important to acknowledge the role of cultural expectations and the perception of
“norms” in the communication process. Script Theory is an academic paradigm employed in the
fields of sociology and cognitive psychology as an explanation for human behavior (McCormick,
1987; Simon and Gagnon, 1986; Schank & Abelson 1977). Within the field of cognitive and
social psychology, script theory is considered analogous to computer programming and places an
emphasis on prior learning dictating future outcomes for an individual’s behavior (McCormick,
1987). In contrast, within sociology, scripts are a set of flexible guidelines, with larger cultural
messages (cultural beliefs) influencing an individuals’ actions (interpersonal scripts) and beliefs
about their actions (intrapsychic scripts; Simon & Gagnon, 2003; McCormick, 1987).
First appearing in the early 1970’s, Sexual Script Theory (SXST) was developed as
response to and rejection of the bio-medical and psychological explanations for sexual behavior
and sought to include contextual factors impact on sexual behavior (Gagnon & Simon, 2003).
Sexual script theory rests on the sociological notion of scripting, where sexually active
individuals have beliefs about the range of behaviors, they can engage in sexually based on
preceding behaviors of their partners (Fantasia, 2011; Rose & Frieze, 1989). Within the SXST
lens, widespread beliefs (i.e., cultural scenarios) around sexuality affect an individual’s actions
(i.e., interpersonal script) and more importantly their fantasies, beliefs, and internal experience
around their sexuality and sexual interactions (i.e., intrapsychic beliefs, Simon & Gagnon. 1986;
13
Widerman, 2015). Contextual factors such as race, age, ethnicity, gender, and sexual orientation,
have all been shown to affect sexual behavior, suggesting nuance and flexibility regarding sexual
scripts is important (Dworkin & O’Sullivan, 2005; Parsons et al., 2012; Simms & Byers, 2013).
In the context of sexual consent research, this framework is used often to explore and
explain behaviors of sexually active individuals. For instance, several studies have examined the
cultural belief of ‘men must initiate sexual activity’ and found that this belief impacts
individuals’ sexual initiation behaviors despite their personal preference or personal beliefs
(Dworkin & O’Sullivan 2005; Hirsch et al., 2019; Jozkowski & Peterson 2013; Jozkowski,
2015). Such findings are used to legitimize a key point of SXST: cultural beliefs affect one’s
sexual behavior (interpersonal scripts) and can override personal desires or beliefs when it comes
to sex practices (intrapsychic scripts). Additional research has gone on to explore and validate
the notion of a gendered (male and female) experience of intrapsychic scripts and subsequent
sexual practices and beliefs (Rosenthal et al., 1998; Ortiz Torres et al. 2003; Peplau 2003).
When considering sexual consent miscommunication violence prevention, SXST and the
notion of gendered intrapsychic scripts have large implications for best practice. For instance,
research into sexual violence prevention, sexual consent communication, and sexual consent
often cite the cultural belief women are expected to act as “gatekeepers” and men as “pleasure
seekers” framing their interactions in relatively set roles (Hirsch et al., 2019; Jozkowski, 2013;
2015; Peplau 2003). However, rather than viewing these as rigid internalized roles, much
research into sexual consent and sexual consent communication notes variation in intrapsychic
scripts of individuals based on gender identity, age, relationship status, race, and sexual interest
[e.g., kink] community membership (Beres & McDonald, 2016; Simms & Byers, 2013).
14
Furthermore, even within the narrow scope of heterosexual interactions, several studies provide
evidence to suggest limitations of generalizability SXST when considering the lived experience
of sexual behaviors and sexual consent communication behaviors of all sexually active people
(Beckmann, 2003; Dworkin & O’Sullivan, 2005; Beres & McDonald, 2016; Simms & Byers,
2013)
Within MSM population, variations in sexual script are apparent and may include the
behaviors of consensual non-monogamy, substance use in during sexual initiation, and sexual
involvement on the first date (Candelas de la Ossa, 2016; Edgar & Fitzpatrick, 1993; Javaid,
2018; Klinkenberg & Rose 1994; Parsons, Starks, Gamarel, & Grov, 2012). Furthermore, when
considering how these script differences may play out in the role of sexual consent
communication, researchers specifically looking into same sex partners note when responding
sexual initiation behaviors MSM report a higher use of nonverbal sexual consent communication
behaviors to indicate their consent when compared to women who have sex with women (WSW;
Beres et al., 2004, Peplau 2003). Additionally, amongst MSM couples, male intrapsychic scripts
(e.g., pleasure-driven scripts) appear to affect the interpersonal scripts and scenarios of MSM
community members regarding sexual and romantic behavior (Parsons, Starks, Gamarel, Grov,
2012). Thus, when thinking about sexual consent and sexual consent communication behaviors
in the MSM community, it is important to understand how cultural scenarios, interpersonal
scripts, and intrapsychic scripts interact in a dynamic fashion to affect consent behavior
communication practices amongst members of this group.
Sexual Consent
15
Further complicating our understanding of sexual consent communication behaviors and
their practice is that there is no clear agreed upon definition of sexual consent (Beres, 2014;
Muehlenhard et al., 2016; Pugh & Becker, 2018). Beres (2007) has put forth that current
literature often engages in spontaneous consent” a process where definitions of sexual consent
are not established by the author, but rather it is assumed the reader shares a common
understanding of consent. Beres then goes on to make the case that, despite its’ central role in
our understanding of sexual violence, within the literature, sexual consent is not defined,
inadequately defined, or defined in ways contradictory to previous definitions (Beres, 2007).
Additionally, while a singular definition of sexual consent and its meaning remains opaque,
equally important is a lack of clarity around the concept of sexual non-consent. Within the scope
of literature some have defined sexual consent as merely the “absence of consent” (Halley,
2016). Within this context, defining sexual consent merely as the absence of consent, engenders
the question of what sexual non-consent and the role sexual non-consent and the communication
of non-consent is plays in our understanding of sexual violence prevention. Muehlenhard et al.’s
(2016) meta-analysis of empirical research on sexual consent notes three main
conceptualizations: sexual consent as an internal state of willingness, sexual consent as an act of
explicitly agreeing to something, and sexual consent as behavior that some else interprets as
willingness.
When considering sexual situations, each of the three conceptualizations put forth by
Muehlenhard et al. (2016) boast strengths and weaknesses as potential basis for the definition of
sexual consent, sexual consent communication behaviors, and the prevention of sexual
miscommunication. For instance, when considering sexual consent as an internal state
16
willingness, this definition notes that sexual consent cannot be objectively defined by solely one
member of the interaction. Therefore, under this premise, sexual consent must clearly involve
both the internal agreement and willingness of one member to do something and the enacting of
behaviors to express that willingness to others successfully (Hickman & Muehlenhard, 1999;
Muehlenhard et al., 2016). Sexual consent as an internal state of willingness places large
emphasis on individual party members demonstrating correct behaviors to communicate their
willingness to engage in sexual activity. This places emphasis on communication behaviors and
lends itself to popular theories of sexual violence and unwanted experiences (at least in some
cases) being due in part to miscommunication of sexual consent (Abbey, 1982, 1987; Fantasia,
2011).
The second broad understanding of sexual consent is an act of explicitly agreeing to
something (Muehlenhard et al. 2016). In sexual situations, this model of sexual consent involves
an explicit verbal agreement between an initiator and respondent to engage in sexual activity.
This perspective of sexual consent most closely aligns with aspirational notions of sexual
consent which seek to have sexual consent explicitly communicated such as affirmative consent
(de La Ossa, 2016; Soble, 2002). As a model of sexual consent, the explicit nature of agreement
employed by these conceptualizations are preferred as they speak to the notion that a lack of
sexual consent (i.e., sexual assault) occurs due to a lack of clear verbal communication between
parties resulting in a miscommunication (Abbey, 1982, 1987; de La Ossa, 2016; Fantasia, 2011).
However, several research findings suggest limitations of conceptualizing sexual consent in this
way, which include the well-documented fact that verbal sexual consent communication
behaviors during sexual consent negotiation is less common than nonverbal sexual consent
17
communication behaviors (Beres et al. 2004; Hall, 1998; Humphreys, 2007; Jozkowski et al.,
2013). Additionally, some have postulated due to the inherent nature of some sexual
relationships (especially heterosexual interactions) that women especially should be able to
convey non-consent with a multitude of means outside of simply saying “no” (Kitzinger & Frith,
1999).
The third and final grouping of sexual consent definitions reviewed by Muehlenhard et al.
(2016) is considering sexual consent as a behavior that someone else interprets as willingness.
When considering sexual consent in this way, the legal notion of “implied consent” is most
applicable to this group. Implied consent suggests that consent is given via a sign or action that
creates a reasonable presumption of acquiescence (Block, 2004). In the context of sexual consent
communication, implied consent relies heavily on the notion of a shared sexual script in which
both actors are familiar with and well-rehearsed in said script. There are several limitations
conceptualizing sexual consent in this way, including the fact subscription to cultural beliefs
(i.e., Cultural scenarios) regarding sexual initiation behaviors and actual sexual behavior (i.e.
interpersonal scripts) can differ among individuals (Beres & McDonald, 2016; Dworkin, &
O'Sullivan, 2005; Simms & Byers, 2013). In summary all three conceptualizations of sexual
consent as outlined by Muehlenhard et al. (2016) provide a unique framework for understanding
sexual consent and thus helping inform policies around effective sexual violence prevention
practices. All three models of sexual consent underscore the importance of effective
communication between parties as being integral to the process of establishing sexual consent.
Sexual Non-Consent
18
When considering the impact of sexual violence on communities, and the role sexual
consent plays in defining instances of sexual violence, it also becomes integral to consider the
role of and our understanding of sexual non-consent. Analysis of sexual consent definitions in
literature have noted that much of the current literature merely refers to sexual violence as
intercourse with a of consent present or “sexual contact achieved without consent” (Beres, 2007;
Halley, p. 262, 2016). Similarly, within the US legal system, there is a long history of examining
the role of force, sexual consent, and sexual non-consent when considering the definition of the
crime of rape (Decker and Boaroni, 2011). Historically many states have included an unfair
burden on those who have experienced an unwanted sexual experience to “prove” an incident
was indeed non-consensual especially in the absence of overt force or violence (Decker and
Baroni 2011). This standard has been used in other crimes within the legal system, with the
presence of “force” being used to delineate between crimes involving similar offenses (e.g.,
larceny vs. robbery, manslaughter vs. murder; Peeler, 2021). However, when considering cases
and instances of sexual violence, intent of the perpetrator is generally outweighed by the impact
of experiences on survivors. Furthermore, as noted by many scholars, the complex nature of
sexual interactions and the “use of force” is not the only indication of an unwanted sexual
experience (Kitzinger & Frith, 1999; Pugh & Becker, 2018). Therefore, when exploring sexual
violence and it’s prevention, its also important for researchers and the public to better understand
the concept of sexual non-consent and the ways in which sexual non-consent is communicated
between parties.
Consent and Violence Prevention
19
As noted above, sexual consent and the communication of sexual consent are important
components of sexual violence prevention. Thus, a large goal of violence prevention programing
is addressing the notion known as implied consent and miscommunications that may result of
that implication. To correct for the ambiguity and miscommunication associated with implied
consent, much attention has been focused on the adoption of laws and education programs that
focus on direct, clear, consistent, communication between parties as exemplified in the practice
of affirmative consent (Curtis & Burnett 2017; Jozkowski & Humphreys, 2014; De León, 2014).
The practice of affirmative consent, which focuses on training individuals to utilize direct,
consistent, and clear verbal communication during sexual activity in order to establish
enthusiastic participation by all parties, has become the primary means of teaching consent
practices--especially on college campuses (Antioch College, 2016; Ali, 2011; De León, 2014).
Affirmative consent is hallmarked by the seven key tenants in its practice which include:
1. Consent must be obtained verbally before there is any sexual contact or conduct.
2. Obtaining consent is an ongoing process in any sexual interaction.
3. If the level of sexual intimacy increases during an interaction… the people
involved need to express their clear verbal consent before moving to that new
level
4. The request for consent must be specific to each act.
5. If you had a particular level of sexual intimacy before with someone, you must
still ask each and every time.
20
6. If someone has initially consented but then stops consenting during a sexual
interaction, she/he should communicate withdrawal verbally and/or through
physical resistance. The other individual(s) must stop immediately.
7. Don’t ever make any assumptions about consent (p.327, Soble 2002).
However, several research findings suggest limitations of such an intervention. In
particular explicit verbal communication behaviors during sexual consent communication is less
common than nonverbal communication behaviors (Beres et al. 2004; Hall, 1998, Jozkowski et
al., 2013; King et al. 2020; Shumlich, & Fisher, 2018). Additionally, some factors such as the
length and duration of a relationship and gender identity of an individual, has been shown to
impact sexual consent communication practices, specifically use of and reliance on verbal
communication behaviors (Humphreys, 2007; Jozkowski & Peterson, 2013; King et. al, 2020;
Willis & Jozkowski, 2019). As outlined above in the seven key tenants, while affirmative
consent acknowledges the role of nonverbal sexual consent communication behaviors it is
decidedly vague on what constitutes these behaviors, especially in comparison to its focus on
verbal sexual consent communication behaviors.
Nonverbal Consent
In addition to observed gender differences in communication behavior patterns, research
has shown that amongst sexual consent communication behaviors, nonverbal sexual consent
communication behaviors are more commonly utilized by all individuals when compared with
verbal communication behaviors (Beres et al. 2004; Hall, 1998; Humphreys, 2007; Jozkowski et
al., 2013). Additionally, despite adoptions by many college campuses, verbal sexual consent
communication behaviors outlined in affirmative consent models rarely reflect the lived
21
experience of students (Curtis & Burnett, 2017; Hall, 1998; Johnson & Hoover, 2015; Jozkowski
& Humphreys, 2014). For example, Curtis and Burnett (2017) found some participants indicated
little to no experience with affirmative consent as a verbal behavior. One female respondent
noted “…But when I come to think of it in the real-world perspective, I think if you’re going
along with the motions and you’re not showing resistance to it and you’re into it, then that’s
consent” (p. 209 Curtis & Burnett, 2017). This statement corroborates with research on sexual
consent communication behaviors amongst college students which notes preference by
participants in the use of non-resistance as a means conveying consent, and a tendency of some
males to continue with a sexual behavior until they encounter a verbal communication behavior
of non-consent (Beres et al., 2004; Camilleri, et al., 2007; Hall, 1998; Humphreys, 2007;
Jozkowski et al., 2013; King et al., 2020).
Many studies (mostly set amongst the college-aged population) have noted the use of and
preference for nonverbal sexual consent communication behaviors, as a part of the sexual
consent practice (Curtis & Burnett, 2017; Hall, 1998; Humphreys, 2007; Jozkowski & Peterson,
2014; King et al. 2020; Kitzinger & Frith, 1999; McCormick, 1979; Shumlich, & Fisher, 2018).
Nonverbal sexual consent communication behaviors such as smiling, nodding, accepting
alcoholic drinks, following a partner to their residence, and genital stimulation, have all been
evaluated to have a range of meanings when conveying sexual consent to partner (Beres et al.
2004, Humphreys & Herold, 2007; Jozkowski & Peterson, 2014; Orchowski et al. 2018).
Additionally, several studies have documented that gender differences in the perception of
communication behaviors of sexual consent exist in heterosexual interactions, with men utilizing
and watching for more nonverbal sexual communication behaviors (i.e., body language) and
22
women utilizing more verbal sexual communication behaviors (Abbey, 1982; Hickman &
Muehlenhard, 1999, Jozkowski & Peterson, 2013; King et. al, 2020; Peplau, 2003). In response
to critiques of the limitations of nonverbal sexual consent communication behavior studies
examining one behavior at a time, King et al. (2020) examined college students’ perceptions of
concurrent/ successive nonverbal sexual consent communication behaviors. Again, results of this
study showed differences in the perception of nonverbal sexual consent communication
behaviors with male participants consistently interpreting successive nonverbal sexual consent
communication behaviors as more indicative of sexual consent then their female peers (King et
al., 2020).
As noted above, there is a significant portion of research which documents the existence
of nonverbal sexual consent communication behaviors. These studies share common themes and
outcomes, including a clear preference for nonverbal sexual consent communication behaviors
amongst participants and male participants utilizing nonverbal sexual consent communication
more ardently than their female counterparts (Beres et al. 2004; Curtis & Burnett, 2017).
Additionally, as many young adults lack access to standardized experiences with sexual
education and education centered sexual consent education, many learn concepts of sexual
consent communication from mainstream depictions of consent in films and pornography (Willis
et al., 2019; 2020). A 2019 study of sexual communication and refusal behaviors depiction in the
media, revealed through the analysis of fifty (50) 2013 films’ depictions of sexual consent
communication behaviors between partners were overwhelmingly nonverbal sexual consent
communication behaviors (Jozkowski et al., 2019). Furthermore, a content analysis of popular
pornographic films done by Willis et al. (2020), reveal similar depictions of nonverbal sexual
23
consent communication behaviors are utilized. Taken together, findings such as these suggest a
mechanism which may lead to the documented preference of college and high school youth
(especially male-identified youth) to rely on and utilize nonverbal sexual consent communication
behaviors (Righi et al. 2019; Nichols Curtis, 2017; King et al. 2020). When taken together, these
findings lend support to the notion of larger societal expectations (cultural scripts) impacting and
influencing individual behaviors (interpersonal scripts) and beliefs/ expectations (intrapsychic
scripts) around nonverbal sexual consent communication behaviors.
Noting the support within the current literature for the existence, preference teaching, of
nonverbal sexual consent communication behaviors- there remains a dearth in our understanding
of these consent communication behaviors. Overwhelmingly, current studies of sexual consent
and sexual consent communication behaviors have been conducted on cisgender, white,
heterosexual, traditionally college-aged students (Beres, 2007; Muehlenhard et al. 2016).
However, several studies have noted the impact of life experiences, especially length of a sexual
partnership and gender socialization, to impact perceptions of sexual consent and sexual consent
communication behaviors (Humphreys, 2007; Willis & Jozkowski, 2019). The concept of sexual
script theory and the heteronormative notion of men being pleasure seekers and women being
gatekeepers play out in many of these majority population studies (Jozkowski, 2017). Taken all
into context, it is important to consider the question of how gender-identity and sexual
orientation may interact and affect conceptions of sexual consent and subscription to and use of
nonverbal sexual consent communication behaviors.
Consent Practices in Underrepresented Communities
24
Overwhelmingly, the North American understanding of sexual consent and sexual
consent communication behaviors have been derived from the experiences of white, college-
educated, and often heterosexual participants (Beres et al., 2004; Jozkowski, et al., 2014;
Humphreys & Brousseau, 2010; Humphreys & Herold, 2007; Ward et al., 2012). This becomes
incredibly significant when we consider the ample documentation for gender specific patterns of
sexual consent communication behaviors, as well as the different patterns of sexual consent
communication behaviors observed specifically within sub-communities (e.g., MSM, S&M, and
WSM; Frankis & Flowers, 2009; Bullock, 2004;).
Specifically, within the MSM community, an historic emphasis by some members of this
community has been placed on nonverbal communication behaviors in order to avoid detection
and persecution by non-community members (Tewksbury, 1996). Historically MSM members
have engaged in nonverbal communication behaviors such as displaying and wearing specific
items of clothing and accessories, physical demonstrations (e.g., tapping of the foot beneath a
stall) and attending designated public spaces (e.g., parks, rest stops, public restrooms) during
designated hours as a means of conveying to other parties their community membership and
potential sexual interest (Tewksbury, 1996). This behavior among MSM community members
has been titled “cruising” and relies heavily on the use of nonverbal sexual consent
communication behaviors including eye contact, pursuit, display, and body contact (Frankis &
Flowers 2009).
As noted in literature on cruising, the specific set of nonverbal communication behaviors
employed in these areas by community members are designed to communicate sexual interest
and sexual consent to community members, but to hold no meaning for non-community
25
members. With this purpose in mind, nonverbal communication behaviors utilized in cruising are
an example of communication behaviors that differ from the overarching sexual script for non-
community members (ie. heterosexuals) as is described in much of the current sexual consent
communication research. More contemporarily, research has also been conducted into the use of
and understanding of “gaydar”, a mechanism by which community members employ a “sixth
sense” to assess and utilize nonverbal and verbal behaviors as a means of identifying potential
sexual community membership and potential sexual/romantic partners (Rule & Alaei, 2016).
Our understanding of sexual consent communication behaviors remains at the heart of
sexual miscommunication and thus some sexual violence prevention efforts. Therefore, it is
crucial to better understand sexual consent communication behaviors within non-majority
populations. For instance, when considering sexual violence within the MSM community,
literature notes a disproportionate experience of sexual violence within this population when
compared to their heterosexual peers (Association of American Universities, 2015; CDC, 2017;
Kosciwet al., 2016). Taken together, the disproportionate amount of violence and community
specific behaviors, underscores the need for a more complete understanding of sexual consent
communication behaviors and concepts of sexual consent within this community.
Delphi
The Delphi method is a multi-round approach to consensus building among experts in a
given field. Historically the Delphi has been termed as a means of refining a groups’ judgement
and has been a way of formalizing the power of group wisdom (Dalkey, 1969). The Delphi
methodology allows for individuals with diverse experiences and expertise to independently
share their knowledge and arrive at a consensus regarding a larger idea (Hasson, Keeney, &
26
McKenna, 2000; Hsu & Sandford, 2007). Additionally, the Delphi method has been successfully
utilized in mental health research regarding best practice and competency, as well as a means of
comparing the knowledge of experts (e.g., researchers, clinicians) and consumers (e.g.,
community stakeholders, patients; Forbes, Hutchison, 2020; Ross, Kelly, Jorm, 2015). As noted
in the exploration of affirmative sexual consent and sexual violence prevention efforts above,
there is a gap between academic best practice (verbal sexual consent communication behaviors)
and the lived experiences of community stake holders regarding their communication of sexual
consent (Curtis & Burnett, 2017). Thus, the Delphi methodology provides an ideal opportunity to
compare and arrive at a group consensus between both researchers and community members
regarding this important topic. Furthermore, considering the Covid-19 pandemic, the Delphi
methodology is an increasingly attractive means of conducting research due to its ability to
collect information and facilitate engagement amongst participants in a socially distant manner
(Khazie, Khan, 2020). Lastly, when working specifically within the sexual minority communities
such as the MSM community, several studies have documented the effectiveness of utilizing
online/ distance methods to engage with this population regarding sexual behaviors and practices
(Bowen, 2005; Ross et al. 2000).
Purpose of the Study
The current study seeks to expand our understanding of sexual consent and nonverbal
sexual consent communication behaviors. Specifically, the current study seeks to address a
dearth in the literature by examining these concepts within the context of two specific
communities--the MSM community and sexuality researchers--by utilizing the Delphi Method
(Dalkey, 1969). Due to the exploratory nature of this study and methodology, there are no
27
expected results or stated hypotheses for this study. Instead, we seek to determine if consensus
between our two groups can be reached on each of the following primary research questions:
1. What are the qualities of sexual consent?
2. What are the elements of sexual non-consent?
3. What are the behaviors associated with sexual consent communication?
4. What are the ways the group interprets and utilizes nonverbal sexual consent
communication behaviors?
28
METHODS
In this study, the Delphi method (Dalkey, 1969) was utilized in order to collect and
analyze data through a multifaceted approach. The Delphi method is a group facilitation method
that is performed in stages, with the ultimate goal being the expert panelists arriving at a
consensus opinion regarding the topic at hand (Jorm, 2015; Linstone & Turoff, 1975). The
current study sought to better understand conceptualizations and experience of panelists
regarding sexual consent and sexual consent communication behaviors.
Participants
In line with the establishment of a community-driven and community-consistent
conceptualization of sexual consent and the behaviors utilized in its communication, participants
for this study were recruited through a snowballing campaign. Eligibility for the current study
included participants identifying with one or more of the following criteria:
1. Identifying as a researcher of human sexuality, sexual violence, or sexual violence
prevention (of any gender identity or sexual orientation).
2. Identifying as a member of the MSM community, who acknowledges having a history of
a sexual experience with another male-identified person.
In addition to the above inclusionary criteria participants were also included based of their
willingness to participate in a multiple-round study, having adequate time and internet access, as
well as their ability to read and write effectively in the English language (Skulmoski et al.,
2007).
29
Our initial panel (Round One) consisted of a total of 35 unique participants who
completed the survey; a complete list of their demographic information can be found in Table 1.
Participants Round One ranged in age from 18 to 55, with a little over 50% of participants
identifying as members of the MSM community. Participants also presented with racial ethnic
diversity with 34% of participants identifying as a member of a racial or ethnic minority group
and 66% identifying as white. Similarly, participants in Round One also presented with various
relationship statuses with 66% indicating they were in a relationship and 34% indicating they
were single. Participants in Round one also presented with a long history of experiences in
higher education with 92% or participants indicating they were a college graduate or had post-
graduate training.
Proceeding to Round Two, 20 respondents completed the second-round survey making
up our panel in this round, a complete list of their demographic information can also be found in
Table 1. Despite some attrition, Round Two’s participants presented with similar representation
of diverse experiences and identities as seen in Round One. Despite attrition of 15 participants,
proportionally demographics of Round Two participants remained largely the same with a
majority of participants identifying as male, and as members of the MSM community.
In the final round, Round Three, a total of 18 participants completed the survey making
up our third-round panel. With an attrition of two, demographics between Round Two and
Round Three largely remain the same. Fifty-one percent of participants (n=18) completed all
three rounds of this Delphi study, the complete list of participants demographics can be found in
Table 1 . A majority of participants in all three rounds identified as MSM community members,
ranging in age (23-55). When compared to Round One, the participants in the final survey are
30
less diverse in their educational experiences with 100% of the final sample having college and
post graduate experience.
Table 1
Delphi Rounds 1,2,3, Panelist Demographics
Table 1
Round 1
Round 2
Demographic
N (35)
%
N (20)
%
N (18)
%
Age
18- 22
1
3%
1
5%
-
23-30
11
32%
8
40%
8
44%
31-40
17
48%
8
40%
8
44%
41- 55
5
14%
2
10%
1
6%
Prefer not to answer
1
3%
1
5%
1
6%
Stakeholder Status
Sexual Researcher (SR)*
13
37%
7
35%
6
33%
MSM Community
member (MSM)
22
63%
13
65%
12
67%
31
Gender identity
Male
27
77%
15
75%
13
72%
Female
8
23%
5
25%
5
28%
Sexual Orientation
Gay
24
70%
14
70%
12
67%
Lesbian
1
3%
1
5%
1
6%
Bisexual
3
8%
2
10%
2
12%
Queer
3
8%
1
5%
1
6%
Heterosexual
4
11%
2
10%
2
10%
Relationship Status
In a relationship
23
66%
12
60%
11
61%
Single and Actively
Seeking a committed
relationship
6
17%
4
20%
4
22%
Single and Casually
Dating
1
3%
-
-
32
Single and not dating
5
14%
4
20%
3
17%
Nature of relationship
N (23)
N (12)
N (11)
Monogamous
19
83%
10
83%
9
81%
Monogamish (mostly
monogamous)
4
17%
2
17%
2
19%
Open
-
-
-
-
-
-
Race
European American
23
66%
16
80%
15
83%
African American
4
11%
2
10%
1
6%
Asian American
3
9%
1
5%
1
6%
Hispanic
1
3%
-
-
Mena
1
3%
-
-
Native Hawaiian
1
3%
-
-
Asian
2
5%
1
5%
1
6%
33
Education
Postgraduate
23
66%
14
70%
13
72%
College Graduate
9
26%
5
25%
5
28%
Some College
2
5%
1
5%
-
Completed 12 years or
HS equivalent
1
3%
-
-
Note. *Two Sexual researchers identify as MSM community members and are noted as (MSR)
Procedure
As noted, the Delphi method is an ideal methodology for gathering consensus amongst
community stakeholders and experts to define a large broad concept such as consent (Forbes,
2020; Jorm, 2015). Following institutional review board approval (UND IRB-201811-094), in
line with the Delphi methodology, participants are recruited and asked to engage in a multi-
round study coordinated by a researcher (Jorm, 2015). In the current study, participants were
identified and recruited through a snowballing methodology by both reviewing current literature
and authorship in sexual violence, sexual violence prevention, and human sexuality, as well as
outreach to MSM specific groups and listservs. Once identified, participants were also invited to
nominate parties who may fit the criteria for the population to also join the study.
Participation in this study was done exclusively online through Qualtrics, which is in line
with both methodological best practice and health board best practice (Ross, 2000). In Round
34
One, participants were provided with either a personalized link or anonymous link which
outlined risks and benefits and were asked to complete a 33-item questionnaire which consisted
of three distinct tasks. Task one of this questionnaire collected participants’ demographic
information. During task two of Round One, participants were asked to provide broad opinions
on the topic of sexual consent via open-ended questions. Finally, task three of Round One asked
participants to review a list of communicative behaviors associated with consent and provide
feedback on that list (Beres 2010). Following the completion of Round One, the open-ended data
collected from the 35 unique participants was qualitatively analyzed and used to construct the
Round Two survey.
In line with the Delphi methodology, data from Round One was collected, analyzed, and
collated to create the Round Two survey (Jorm,2015). During Round Two the first task asked
participants to review and provide feedback regarding their level of agreement with their peers'
qualitative responses (31 in all) to the question “How would you describe sexual consent (Q48)”.
More specifically, after reviewing an individual Round One qualitative response, participants
were asked to note their level of agreement with their peers’ statement on a 5-point Likert scale 1
(This does not adequately describe sexual consent) to 5 (This very much describes sexual
consent).
The second task of Round Two invited participants to provide narrative feedback on the
six broad qualities of consent that were derived (by the researcher) from participant responses to
Q48 in Round One. The third task of Round Two asked respondents to rank order the six broad
qualities of consent in order from importance (ranking 1-6, with 1 as most important). In the
fourth and final task of Round Two, participants were asked to provide their thoughts on sexual
35
non-consent through a response to a set of open-ended questions which included “How would
you describe sexual non-consent? 20 participants from Round One (57%) completed Round
Two of this three-round study.
Round Three of this three-round study consisted of four distinct tasks that participants
were asked to complete. The first task consisted of asking participants to review their peers’
responses to the Q72 of Round Two and provide feedback regarding their level of agreement
with their peers’ conceptualization of sexual non-consent. Specifically, participants were asked
to note their level of agreement with peers’ statements on a 5-point Likert scale 1 (This does not
adequately describe sexual non-consent) to 5 (This very much describes sexual non-consent).
The second task of Round Three asked participants to review five elements of sexual
non-consent which were derived (by the researcher) from the groups’ open-ended responses to
Q72 in Round Two Following their review of these five elements of sexual non-consent,
participants were again asked to provide feedback and to rank order the elements of sexual non-
consent in terms of impact (ranking 1-5, with one as the most impactful). In the third task of
Round Three, participants were provided with their personal rank ordering of consent qualities
collected in Round Two and asked to compare their personal positioning with the groups’
collective rankings of consent qualities. After comparing these rankings, participants were asked
to confirm their personal rankings.
The final task of Round Three asked participants to review qualities of sexual consent
and elements of sexual non-consent as determined by the group. Following their review of these
items, participants were asked to review and sort a list of nonverbal behaviors of sexual consent
36
communication derived from the group’s responses in round one and sort them into groups based
on their perceived function (i.e., do the individual items represent Consent Giving Behavior,
Consent Seeking Behavior, Interchangeable Consent Behavior, Ambiguous consent Behavior, or
Consent Refusal Behavior). For Round Three, 18 of the 20 respondents to Round Two (90%)
completed all four tasks.
Data Analysis
In line with the Delphi methodology, a mixed method approach was used to assess the
qualitative and quantitative data collected as part of this study (Skulmoski, Hartman, & Krahn,
2007). A key element of Delphi studies is the reaching of consensus on an issue by a panel of
experts (Jorm, 2015). Formally there are no guidelines for establishing consensus within a given
delphi study, therefore, in the current study a novel and new three-tiered system of group
agreement was established in order to better determine the level of and reveal the nuances of
agreement amongst participants (Jorm 2015; Nair et al. 2011; Waggoner, Carline, Durning,
2016). The top tier of this three-tier system was termed Major Consensus, which was met when
90% of participants of the study were in agreement on an issue as demonstrated by their
responses on the Likert scale. The second tier was termed Consensus and was reached when 70%
of participants indicating agreement on a singular issue as demonstrated by their responses on the
Likert scale. The final tier was termed Endorsement and consists of 50 % of participants
indicating agreement on an issue as demonstrated by their responses on the Likert scale.
Similarly, when examining qualitative data in Delphi studies there are few guidelines in
place for data analysis, thus for this study a grounded constructivist framework was applied for
the data analysis with the adoption of an outside reader to increase trustworthiness (Chamaz,
2008; Skulmoski, et al. 2007; Krippendorff, 2015). The grounded constructivist framework was
37
selected for its usefulness in analyzing data for major themes, as well as its emphasis on the role
of the researcher and the lens by which they view and interpret the data (Chamaz, 2008). In the
current study, the author thoroughly read the data before attempting to code responses (with both
open coding and axial coding), kept a journal (“memoing”) to utilize the reflective process,
utilized a reader to provide an additional point of view, and provided participants with
opportunities to provide feedback regarding the coding process (Chamaz, 2008).
Data Analysis Round One
During Round One, qualitative responses to question 48 were collected and analyzed
using a grounded constructivist framework. Responses were initially de-identified by the first
author and reviewed for key elements in the responses. Key elements included overall ideas or
statements indicated by a participant as being central to their notion of sexual consent (Chamaz,
2008). Following the highlighting of key elements, seven overarching codes were developed, and
responses were sorted along those codes for subsequent analysis. Utilizing the constructivist
grounded framework (Chamaz, 2008), responses were analyzed for content to derive six overall
qualities.
Data Analysis Round Two
Following the collection of qualitative data in round one, the group was asked to establish
consensus regarding the descriptions of sexual consent provided by the group, utilizing a five-
point Likert scale. The mean and standard deviation was used as a mechanism to indicate
participants' overall level of agreeance to the sexual consent descriptions and help inform
whether consensus was reached. In the current study, consensus was defined according to the
percentage of participants who fell in agreement regarding a description. Major Consensus was
defined as at least 90% of participants selecting a 4 or 5 on the Likert scale, Consensus was
38
defined as at least 70% of respondents selecting a 4 or 5 on the Likert scale, and an Endorsement
was at least 50% of participants selecting a 4 or 5 on the Likert scale (Bono, 2017; Jorm 2015;
Nair, Aggarwal, Khanna, 2011; Waggoner, Carline, Durning, 2016).
Finally, data was collected from Round Two to help construct the Round 3 survey.
Participant’s initial rank order positions of six qualities of sexual consent were collected and
averaged to construct an initial ranking. Additionally, during the second round, 20 qualitative
responses were gathered to question 72 regarding elements of sexual non-consent. Again,
utilizing a grounded constructivist framework, responses were analyzed and coded into five
elements of sexual non-consent (Table 10).
Data Analysis Round Three
Participants in Round Three were asked to review their peers’ response to question 72 regarding
descriptions of sexual non-consent via a Likert scale. Means and standard deviation were used to
help determine which tier of group agreement was met (e.g., Major Consensus ≥90%, Consensus
≥70%, Endorsement ≥ 50%). Additionally, participants in round three were asked to review their
initial individual rank order positions of sexual consent qualities and re-rank these qualities after
comparing them to the group aggregate. Again, results from these rankings were collected and
analyzed by the percentage of group agreement. Next, participants were also asked to review and
rank a list of elements of non-consent during this task which was analyzed for consensus based
on the three-tiered group agreement system.
The final task of Round Three asked participants to sort a list of nonverbal consent
behaviors among six categories of usage (Consent Giving Behavior, Consent Seeking Behavior,
Interchangeable Consent Behavior, Ambiguous consent Behavior, Consent Refusal Behavior,
Unused behavior). The tiered system of agreement was utilized sorted a given behavior into a
39
usage category. Additionally, a chi-square analysis was run to examine for significant differences
in sorting of behavior usage by group membership (SR vs. MSM).
40
RESULTS
Across three rounds, the current study gathered data from participants regarding their
conceptualization of sexual consent and consent communication behaviors. Participants
consisted of researchers of human sexuality, sexual violence, and sexual violence prevention, as
well as MSM community members. Throughout each round both qualitative and quantitative
data was gathered in order to better understand sexual consent and its’ nonverbal communication
practices. The current study sought to explore three topics, including sexual consent, sexual non-
consent, and nonverbal communication behaviors associated with sexual consent. In the
following results section, each of the subjects is discussed in depth covering each subject and its
exploration through all three rounds of the study.
Sexual Consent Results
In Round One, participants were asked to respond to the open-ended prompt of “How
would you describe sexual consent?” participants provided 34 unique qualitative descriptions of
sexual consent. These responses were analyzed with three responses being consolidated due to
similar content for a total of 31 descriptions (Charmaz, 2008, see left panel of Table 2).The 31
descriptions of sexual consent formed the central data for open and axial coding. Six broad
categories emerged from the coding of the 31 descriptions and these were used to construct six
broad qualities of sexual consent shown below:
1. Sexual consent should be mutual between all parties.
2. Sexual consent should be permission granting/ affirming.
41
3. Sexual consent should be confirmed via verbal and non-verbal behaviors.
4. Sexual consent should be freely/given without influence.
5. Sexual consent should be ongoing.
6. Sexual consent should be reversible/ revocable.
Sexual Consent quality one, was derived from Broad category (1) Mutual Agreement.
Descriptions coded with Mutual Agreement noted the need for a mutuality of agreement or
consent between parties. Seventeen responses were coded with Mutual Agreement, a sample
response coded with Mutual Agreement was: “Sexual consent is mutual agreement to engage in a
sexual activity while setting specific boundaries”.
Broad category (2) Permission Granting/ Affirming was utilized when a description noted
an element of permission granting or affirmation behaviors as part of the consent process. This
code was utilized a total of 15 times and is best exemplified by the description “Sexual consent is
the active and ongoing affirmation that sexual activity is desired or welcomed. Affirmation
includes verbal and nonverbal communication.”
Broad category (3) is Confirmed via verbal and non-verbal behaviors, which was used
when Response notes an explicit need for verbal or nonverbal confirmation among parties. A
total of six descriptions utilized this code and it is best exemplified by the description:
“Consistent with muehlenhard et als review paper, an explicit agreement to do something. can be
communicated verbally or nonverbally”.
42
The next broad category (4) Freely Given/ Without Influence was used when a
description noted that lack of coercion, substance induced influence is necessary when giving
consent. A total of six descriptions were coded with Freely Given/ Without influence and this
category is best exemplified by the description: “Sexual consent is a mutual agreement between
2 or more people to engage in sexual activity - without coercion or compensation. All people
must be capable of consenting and agreeing.”
Broad Category (5) Ongoing was utilized with descriptions which noted consent is a
continuous process and must be present for the duration of activity. Ongoing was used for a total
of six descriptions and is best exemplified by the statement: “When adult confirms…This
confirmation must be present for the duration of the sexual activity…”.
Broad Category (6) Reversible, was used with descriptions that noted that consent has
elements that are reversible or revocable. This code was used a total of four times and is best
exemplified by the description: “Permission to engage sexual activity from the other person(s).
This permission can be rescinded at any time before, during, or after.”
In Round Two, participants reviewed the 31 original descriptions of sexual consent
collected in Round One, as well as the six qualities of sexual consent. In the first part of Round
Two, participants rated the 31 original descriptions of sexual consent on a five-point Likert scale
with 1 being “This does not adequately describe consent” and 5 “This very much describes
consent”. Table 2 records the mean, standard deviation, and variation of the groups’ responses to
each description of sexual consent. Additionally, Table 2 notes the consensus percentage for
each response, specifically the number of participants who indicate a four or five on the Likert
43
scale. Of the 31 descriptions rated in round two, 24 descriptions (77%) met some form of group
agreement. Descriptions number ten and seven also met group consensus with majority rating
these statements negatively with a 1 or 2 on the Likert scale and thus were not included in the
analysis of table 2. Of those 22 descriptions with a positive level of agreement, 7 descriptions
(31%) reached a Major Consensus with 90% of participants indicating a four or five on the
Likert scale; 5 descriptions (22%) reached Consensus with 70% of participants indicating a four
or five on the Likert scale; and 10 descriptions (45%) reached an endorsement of the group with
50% of the group indicating a four or five on the Likert scale. Finally, Table 2 also records
limited demographic information of the participants who make up the consensus response.
Specifically, means, SD and consensus percentage of stakeholder groups are recorded in Table
Two denoted by their abbreviation.
Table 2,
Round 2, Level of Agreement to Descriptions of Consent 1(This does not adequately describe
consent) to 5 (This very much describes consent) with Mean, Standard Deviation, and Variance
Table 2
Description
M
SD
Conse
nsus
%
1. I imagine 2 formal variations in
consent:1) Responding in the affirmative to
a suggestion for sexual activity (responding
by saying “yes” to a verbal, physical, or
otherwise suggestive (look, gesture, body or
body-part positioning or repositioning)
request to engage in sexual activity); or2)
Panel
4.0
1.07
80%
(n=16)
SR
4.0
.70
80%
44
Initiating the above expressed suggestion to
engage in sexual activity. However, a
caveat I would like to mention is that
perceived consent from one party may not
be the actual expression of consent by the
other. **
(n = 4)
MSM
4.0
1.2
77%
(n=
10)
MSR
4.0
0
100%
(n = 2)
2. Two adults confirming they are
comfortable with engaging in sexual
activity.**
Panel
4.1
.78
75%
(n=15)
SR
4.0
0
100%
(n = 5)
MSM
4.3
.85
77%
(n=
10)
MSR
3.0
0
-
3. Having permission and agreement from a
partner(s) to engage in a sexual act that is
not coerced or influenced in any one.***
Panel
4.3
.74
90%
(n=
18)
SR
4.4
.89
80%
(n = 4)
MSM
4.38
.76
80%
(n =
45
12)
MSR
4.0
0
100%
(n = 2)
4. I would describe sexual consent as the
effective communication of ongoing,
affirming, equitable, relational decisions
regarding sexual choice among partners of
free-will. ***
Panel
4.55
.82
90%
(n=
18)
SR
5
0
100%
(n = 5)
MSM
4.30
.94
84%
(n =
11)
MSR
5.0
0
100%
(n = 2)
5. Sexual consent is an ongoing process to
engage in sexual activities with another
person. Sexual consent can be withdrawn at
any time and for any reason. Some
individuals are unable to give sexual consent
due to the undue influence of power to obtain
that consent (e.g. children, individuals in
police custody, people with advanced
dementia). ***
Panel
4.6
.81
90%
(n=
18)
SR
5.0
0
100%
(n = 5)
MSM
4.69
.63
92%
(n =
12)
46
MSR
3.5
2.12
50%
(n = 1)
6. Mutual and unambiguous understanding
between all parties that a sexual activity is
desired and being entered into and
participated in without coercion,
exploitation, or abuse. ***
Panel
4.5
.75
95%
(n=19)
SR
4.4
.54
100%
(n = 5)
MSM
4.61
.63
92%
(n =
12)
MSR
4.5
..5
100%
(n = 2)
ix
7. consistent with muehlenhard et als review
paper, an explicit agreement to do
something. can be communicated verbally
or nonverbally
Panel
3.1
1.2
35%
(n= 7)
SR
3
1.58
40%
(n = 2)
MSM
3.15
1.34
38%
(n = 5)
MSR
3.0
0
-
8. All parties being of sound mind to give
verbal permission to engage in any activity
believed to be, or identified as, sexual
Panel
3.5
1.1
45%
(n=9)
SR
2.4
.8
-
MSM
3.92
1.03
61%
(n = 8)
MSR
3.5
.5
50%
(n = 1)
9. Sexual consent is when both people agree
on a specific act when intimate with each
other.
*
Panel
3.3
.87
40%
(n=8)
SR
2.8
.75
20%
(n =1)
10
MSM
3.92
1.34
46%
(n = 6)
MSR
3.5
.5
50%
(n = 1)
10. A verbal agreement between two
consenting adults
Panel
2.5
1.2
25%
(n=5)
SR
1.6
.75
60%
(n = 3)
MSM
2.92
1.32
15%
(n = 2)
MSR
2.5
.5
-
11. Permission to engage sexual activity
from the other person(s). This permission
can be rescinded at any time before, during,
or after. **
Panel
4.05
.88
75%
(n
=15)
SR
3.6
.24
60%
(n = 3)
MSM
4.46
.66
92%
(n =
12)
11
MSR
2.5
.5
-
12. Sexual consent is mutual agreement to
engage in a sexual activity while setting
specific boundaries *
Panel
3.9
.78
65%
(n=13)
SR
3.8
.83
80%
(n = 4)
MSM
4.0
.81
61%
(n = 8)
MSR
3.5
.5
50%
(n = 1)
ix
13. psychological, emotional, and spiritual
permission delivered in an active process for
one or more sexual activities to occur. *
Panel
3.6
1.1
60%
(n=
12)
SR
3.4
1.5
60%
(n = 3)
MSM
3.76
1.0
61%
(n = 8)
MSR
3.5
.50
50%
(n = 1)
14. Sexual consent is when someone
knowingly participates in and allows sexual
activity with another person.
Panel
3.5
1.1
45 %
(n= 9)
SR
3.6
1.1
60%
(n = 3)
MSM
3.7
1.1
46%
(n = 6)
MSR
2.0
0
-
15. Sexual consent is the active and ongoing
affirmation that sexual activity is desired or
welcomed. Affirmation includes verbal and
nonverbal communication. ***
Panel
4.6
.59
95%
(n=19)
SR
4.6
.54
100%
(n = 5)
10
MSM
4.53
.66
92%
(n =
12)
MSR
5.0
0
100%
(n = 2)
16. When all participants of a sexual
encounter want the encounter to happen at
that time.
Panel
3.0
1.1
40 %
(n=8)
SR
3
.89
40%
(n = 2)
MSM
3.15
1.28
46%
(n = 6)
MSR
2.5
.5
-
17. When the other person allows to have
sex with you
Panel
2.2
1.1
15%
(n=3)
SR
1.4
.49
-
MSM
2.53
1.19
23%
(n = 3)
MSR
1.5
.5
-
11
18. When both parties give a verbal
confirmation of what is ok and not ok to
engage with. This confirmation can be
reinforced or revoked at any time.
Panel
3.3
1.3
40%
(n=8)
SR
2.2
.75
-
MSM
3.76
1.30
61%
(n = 8)
MSR
3.0
1.0
50%
(n = 1)
19. Consent is between two people. As long
as 2 people say yes then it is good to go.
Consent can be withdrawn at any time.
Anyone who has had some mind altering
substances or are unconscious will
automatically not give consent.
Panel
3.0
1.2
30%
(n= 6)
SR
2.2
.75
-
MSM
3.46
1.2
46%
(n = 6)
MSR
2.5
.5
-
20. Agreement on terms of what areas to
touch and what types of sexual contact to
use (oral, anal, etc).*
Panel
3.2
1.1
50%
(n=
10)
SR
2.8
1.17
40%
(n = 2)
12
MSM
3.46
1.05
54%
(n = 7)
MSR
2.5
1.5
50%
(n = 1)
21. Freely given agreement to sexual
activities *
Panel
3.3
1.5
50 %
(n=10)
SR
3.2
1.30
40%
(n = 2)
MSM
3.38
1.5
54%
(n = 7)
MSR
3.0
2.82
50%
(n = 1)
22. Sexual consent is if all participating
members agree to the sexual activities being
presented. Sexual consent cannot be
obtained when one or more party members
are under the influence of drugs, alcohol, or
altered mental health state. *
Panel
3.6
1.4
60%
(n=12)
SR
2.8
.1.30
23%
(n = 3)
MSM
4.07
1.03
69%
(n = 9)
MSR
3.0
0
-
13
23. When an adult confirms that a specific
sexual activity is okay with them, and this
confirmation is shared by all parties
involved in the sexual activity. All involved
parties must actually be able to knowingly
confirm their approval of involvement in the
sexual activity. This confirmation must be
present for the duration of the sexual
activity. Any party involved in the sexual
activity may choose to no longer participate
at any time during the sexual activity, at
which time they would no longer give their
consent or confirmation to continue. All
involved parties must agree to these terms
for the sexual activity to be considered
consensual. ***
Panel
4.25
.96
95%
(n=19)
SR
3.2
1.09
100%
(n = 5)
MSM
4.61
.65
92%
(n =
12)
MSR
4.5
.5
100%
(n = 2)
24. I would describe consent as the
agreement of two interested parties to
engage in agreed upon sexual activities.
Panel
3.4
1.0
45%
(n=9)
SR
3.0
.89
40%
(n = 2)
MSM
3.69
1.10
54%
(n = 7)
MSR
3.0
0
-
25. It's when the other person clearly
indicates they wish a sexual act to happen.
This could be verbal, written or through
body language (although that's a harder line
to define).*
Panel
3.4
1.1
55%
(n=11)
SR
3
.89
40%
14
(n = 2)
MSM
3.61
1.26
61%
(n = 8)
MSR
3.5
.5
50%
(n = 1)
26. A fluid and reversible assertion that all
members of a sexual scene are present and
readily willing to engage in the acts being
proposed. *
Panel
4.0
.88
75%
(n=15)
SR
3.8
.83
60%
(n = 3)
MSM
4.2
.92
84%
(n =
11)
MSR
3.5
.5
50%
(n = 1)
27. Establishing a clear agreement that both
partners would like to have a sexual
encounter.*
Panel
3.6
.88
55%
(n=11)
SR
3.4
.80
60%
(n = 3)
MSM
3.7
.92
61%
(n = 8)
15
MSR
3.0
0
-
28. Sexual consent is a mutual agreement
between 2 or more people to engage in
sexual activity - without coercion or
compensation. All people must be capable
of consenting and agreeing. **
Panel
3.9
1.2
70%
(n=
14)
SR
4.2
1.09
60%
(n = 3)
MSM
3.9
1.44
77%
(n=
10)
MSR
3.5
.50
50%
(n = 1)
29. [s]exual consent is both a cognitive
decision and a behavioural display (verbal
or nonverbal), signaling a willingness (free
from coercion/ incapacitation) to engage in
sexual activity. ***
Panel
4.2
.61
90%
(n=
18)
SR
4.8
.44
100%
(n = 5)
MSM
4.07
.49
92%
(n =
12)
MSR
3.5
.50
50%
(n = 1)
16
30.Sexual Consent is a verbal or nonverbal
agreement between two adults to engage in
sexual acts with one another. Sexual consent
can be either clearly defined or implied
depending on the setting of the encounter
and habits of the individuals. *
Panel
3.3
.92
50%
(n=
10)
SR
2.6
.54
40%
(n = 2)
MSM
3.69
.85
61%
(n = 8)
MSR
2.5
.25
-
31.Sexual consent is the permission by
another to engage in a sexual act. The
consent involves complete choice from the
other without substance coercion or force.
**
Panel
3.9
1.0
75%
(n=
15)
SR
4.4
.89
80%
(n = 4)
MSM
3.92
1.15
77%
(n=
10)
MSR
3.0
1.0
50%
(n = 1)
*Note. Level of agreement * Endorsement = > 50% participant agreement ; ** Consensus =
>70% participant agreement *** Major Consensus = >90% participant agreement
17
As part of Round Two, qualitative feedback was also collected from participants
regarding their reactions to peers’ descriptions of sexual consent collected in Round One. Table
3 contains the qualitative comments made by participants in response to peers’ descriptions of
sexual consent. Comments were sorted by description reference number and according to
stakeholder status. Table 3 highlights the diverse opinions amongst participants, particularly
around the use jargon and the importance of elements of sexual consent.
Table 3
Round 2, Task 1, Qualitative Feedback to participants descriptions of Sexual Consent
Table 3
How would you
describe sexual
consent?
Qualitative Comments
Sexual Researcher
MSM
Both
1. I imagine 2
formal variations in
consent:1)
Responding in the
affirmative to a
suggestion for
sexual activity
(responding by
saying “yes” to a
verbal, physical, or
otherwise
suggestive (look,
gesture, body or
body-part
the "initiating" part
of number 1
confuses me a little
bit.
The first statement was very
confusing. The first half made
total sense to me but the
second half starting with "I
would like to add a caveat"
lost me. Even after rereading
it, I still don't quite understand
it.
I found the wording and
grammar of many of these
responses confusing and so
I'm not fully sure I understood
the point you were trying to
18
positioning or
repositioning)
request to engage in
sexual activity);
or2) Initiating the
above expressed
suggestion to
engage in sexual
activity. However,
a caveat I would
like to mention is
that perceived
consent from one
party may not be
the actual
expression of
consent by the
other. **
get across. Particularly
number one which has a
number of parentheticals.
2. Two adults
confirming they are
comfortable with
engaging in sexual
activity. **
Honest question
about #2 - can
minors "consent" to
one another in some
form if they're both
minors? That's what
threw me off,
though I'm unsure
of the answer, so
maybe it's fine as it
is.
Statement two is the most
basic of the five but all five
allow for a clear picture of
what consent should be.
Comments 2, 3, and 4 do not
mention specific means of
communicating consent
between participants.
3. Having
permission and
agreement from a
partner(s) to engage
in a sexual act that
is not coerced or
influenced in any
one. ***
I like 3 and 4
because they
capture it so
snappily.
I especially agree with the
emphasis in 3, 4, 5 about the
importance of ongoing
communication and the ability
to withdraw consent, as well
as the importance of equity
and the role that power
dynamics play.
Comments 2, 3, and 4 do not
mention specific means of
19
communicating consent
between participants.
3, 4, and 5 each bring in pieces
of equity, free-will, or power
dynamics that must be taken
into consideration of
participating parties, which I
also see as essential to
consent.
4. I would describe
sexual consent as
the effective
communication of
ongoing, affirming,
equitable, relational
decisions regarding
sexual choice
among partners of
free-will.***
4 and 5 include that
consent is an
ongoing process,
which I see as a key
component in the
definition.
I like 3 and 4
because they
capture it so
snappily.
No 4 does not include
reference to accepting sexual
activity, it simply states that
sexual choice is
communicated. This sounds
more like declaring
preferences than actually
engaging in sex.
I especially agree with the
emphasis in 3, 4, 5 about the
importance of ongoing
communication and the ability
to withdraw consent, as well
as the importance of equity
and the role that power
dynamics play.
3, 4, and 5 each bring in pieces
of equity, free-will, or power
dynamics that must be taken
into consideration of
participating parties, which I
also see as essential to
consent.
Comments 2, 3, and 4 do not
mention specific means of
communicating consent
between participants.
20
5. Sexual consent is
an ongoing process
to engage in sexual
activities with
another person.
Sexual consent can
be withdrawn at
any time and for
any reason. Some
individuals are
unable to give
sexual consent due
to the undue
influence of power
to obtain that
consent (e.g.
children,
individuals in
police custody,
people with
advanced
dementia). ***
5. I like the notion
that consent can be
withdrawn and
limits of ability to
give consent.
4 and 5 include that
consent is an
ongoing process,
which I see as a key
component in the
definition.
No. 5 States that consent is the
process of engaging in sexual
activities and leaves out
reference to approving sexual
acts.
3, 4, and 5 each bring in pieces
of equity, free-will, or power
dynamics that must be taken
into consideration of
participating parties, which I
also see as essential to
consent.
I especially agree with the
emphasis in 3, 4, 5 about the
importance of ongoing
communication and the ability
to withdraw consent, as well
as the importance of equity
and the role that power
dynamics play.
Referencing
answers 1-5
Some are more detailed than
others. Some...I just don't
understand the words to be
frank. I almost interpreted the
rating as a ranking of sorts
("Oh, that one seems more
true").
6. Mutual and
unambiguous
understanding
between all parties
that a sexual
activity is desired
and being entered
into and
participated in
without coercion,
#6 - Was very strong
definition!
21
exploitation, or
abuse. ***
7. Consistent with
muehlenhard et als
review paper, an
explicit agreement
to do something.
can be
communicated
verbally or
nonverbally
Comment 7 makes reference
to a source I am not familiar
with so I cannot fully
determine whether I agree
with the comment.
And 7 is all-around a nope for
me.
No 7 - I am not aware of
research by Muehlenhard.
#7 - I have no idea what
muehlenhard et als is so I feel
like I cannot judge it fairly!
8. All parties being
of sound mind to
give verbal
permission to
engage in any
activity believed to
be, or identified as,
sexual
Comments 8 and 10 limit the
expression of consent to
spoken word. Based on
experience, there are instances
when two or more people
agree to engage in sexual
activity without a single word
spoken. These comments do
not adequately describe
consent as they exclude non-
verbal gestures and body
language which are important
mediums of communication.
8 doesn't go far enough -
"sound mind" doesn't cover
coercion, power.
22
8. Excludes nonverbal
consent. "Sound mind" is not
explained.
9. Sexual consent is
when both people
agree on a specific
act when intimate
with each other.
9 is pretty good but
doesn't mention
coercion or power.
9 does not account for power
dynamics.
No 9 does not include
reference to sexual partners
being able to give consent (i.e.
children or coercion pressure)
9. Exclusionary to
polyamorous folx
10. A verbal
agreement between
two consenting
adults
10 asserts that
consent must be
verbal, which is not
always the case.
10. What does it
mean to be a
consenting adult?
Comments 8 and 10 limit the
expression of consent to
spoken word. Based on
experience, there are instances
when two or more people
agree to engage in sexual
activity without a single word
spoken. These comments do
not adequately describe
consent as they exclude non-
verbal gestures and body
language which are important
mediums of communication.
10s [sic] just naming the thing
as the definition.
No 10 on references a verbal
agreement. There are non
verbal ways to give consent,
specifically physical touch.
23
Referencing
answers 6-10
It seems that due to
several reasons,
verbal agreements
may often be
important, but are
complicated. Some
people do not have
verbal ability, some
are not able to hear,
and some sexual
acts may make
verbal discussion
difficult. And while
I don't disagree with
the spirit of verbal
consent, and often
consider it
important, I think
that it's the
underlying shared
communication
process among
individuals that is
most important.
11. Permission to
engage sexual
activity from the
other person(s).
This permission can
be rescinded at any
time before, during,
or after.
**
These are all pretty
good. I gave # 11,
12, and 13 ratings
of 4 instead of 5
because now that
I've seen the
strength of
definitions that
explicitly address
coercion and power,
definitions that
don't make note of
that feel lacking
Comments 11 and 12 are
vague and not specific in
describing how consent is
communicated between
participants.
No 11 and 12 exclude the
requirement that those giving
consent must be capable of
giving consent.
24
12. Sexual
consent is mutual
agreement to
engage in a sexual
activity while
setting specific
boundaries *
These are all pretty
good. I gave # 11,
12, and 13 ratings
of 4 instead of 5
because now that
I've seen the
strength of
definitions that
explicitly address
coercion and power,
definitions that
don't make note of
that feel lacking
Comments 11 and 12 are
vague and not specific in
describing how consent is
communicated between
participants.
No 11 and 12 exclude the
requirement that those giving
consent must be capable of
giving consent.
Establishing boundaries per 12
is important.
Strongest page so far! I like
#12 discussing boundaries!
13. psychological,
emotional, and
spiritual permission
delivered in an
active process for
one or more sexual
activities to occur.
*
These are all pretty
good. I gave # 11,
12, and 13 ratings
of 4 instead of 5
because now that
I've seen the
strength of
definitions that
explicitly address
coercion and power,
definitions that
don't make note of
that feel lacking.
It is hard to discern the
differences between many of
these things. I wouldn't even
know how to describe
"spiritual permission", perhaps
someone's definition of that is
something I would agree is
part of sexual consent.
No 13 does not include
acknowledgement of consent
to your partner and excludes
communication.
14. Sexual consent
is when someone
knowingly
participates in and
allows sexual
activity with
another person.
25
15. Sexual consent
is the active and
ongoing affirmation
that sexual activity
is desired or
welcomed.
Affirmation
includes verbal and
nonverbal
communication.
***
Referencing 11-15
The idea of
permission or
allowing one partner
to engage in sexual
activity seems
potentially passive
to me, and active
consent seems
essential.
Referencing 11-15.
16. When all
participants of a
sexual encounter
want the encounter
to happen at that
time.
16 is technically
100% accurate
(actually, the most
accurate of any of
these!!) but if only,
if only we could
KNOW when
someone wants
something without
all this
verbal/nonverbal
stuff..
Comment 16 does not provide
specifics of how consent is
communicated between
participants..
No 16, 17, and 20 exclude
person being able to give
consent.
16. Needs more information
regarding communication or
what is meant by a sexual
"encounter."
16 and 17 do not give an
adequate description of how
consent is given and what it
does.
26
17. When the
other person allows
to have sex with
you
17 is....strange.
Comment 17, because a
person allows you to have sex
with them does not
automatically mean they want
the sexual activity to occur. A
participant could be coerced,
under the influence of an
outside force, or not of sound
mind.
No 16, 17, and 20 exclude
person being able to give
consent.
17. This definition feels too
"black and white" to describe
consent - nothing about it
being an ongoing process,
establishing boundaries, nor
does it explain what is meant
by sex and how that can vary.
17 is missing the importance
of equity and power dynamics.
#17 - Is very concerning and I
think could use some more
detail. This sounds very one
sided
16 and 17 do not give an
adequate description of how
consent is given and what it
does.
27
18. When both
parties give a
verbal confirmation
of what is ok and
not ok to engage
with. This
confirmation can be
reinforced or
revoked at any
time.
Comments 18 and 19 limit
communication to verbal
medium, leaving out non-
verbal means of
communication.
18 is delineating do's and
DONTs. the dont's don't seem
to be a requirement of consent
but are advisable.
In response to #18, consent
does not always have to be
verbal.
No 18 only allows for verbal
consent. Non verbal consent
exists.
19. Consent is
between two
people. As long as
2 people say yes
then it is good to
go. Consent can be
withdrawn at any
time. Anyone who
has had some mind
altering substances
or are unconscious
will automatically
not give consent.
19. Only applies to
monogamous
individuals.
Comments 18 and 19 limit
communication to verbal
medium, leaving out non-
verbal means of
communication.
No 19 only references
substances as blocking ability
to give consent but there are
other causes (Age, mental
health, etc.)
19 highlights use of
substances as a factor.
20. Agreement on
terms of what areas
to touch and what
types of sexual
contact to use (oral,
anal, etc). *
Comment 20 is not specific on
how communication is
accomplished.
No 16, 17, and 20 exclude
person being able to give
consent.
28
Referencing 16-20
The grammar, informality, and
spelling makes this hard to do.
Sex sometimes
involves more than
two people and
consent should not
be definitionally
limited to any
number of
participants.
21. Freely given
agreement to sexual
activities. *
Comments 21-24 are not
specific on the means by
which the communication for
consent occurs
22. Sexual consent
is if all
participating
members agree to
the sexual activities
being presented.
Sexual consent
cannot be obtained
when one or more
party members are
under the influence
of drugs, alcohol,
or altered mental
health state. *
Comments 21-24 are not
specific on the means by
which the communication for
consent occurs
23. When an adult
confirms that a
specific sexual
activity is okay
with them, and this
confirmation is
shared by all parties
involved in the
sexual activity. All
involved parties
must actually be
23 is very inclusive
and reflects the
complexity of
consent (that it must
be freely given, can
be revoked at any
time, and that all
parties must be able
to knowingly give
their consent).
Comments 21-24 are not
specific on the means by
which the communication for
consent occurs
29
able to knowingly
confirm their
approval of
involvement in the
sexual activity.
This confirmation
must be present for
the duration of the
sexual activity. Any
party involved in
the sexual activity
may choose to no
longer participate at
any time during the
sexual activity, at
which time they
would no longer
give their consent
or confirmation to
continue. All
involved parties
must agree to these
terms for the sexual
activity to be
considered
consensual. ***
24. I would
describe consent as
the agreement of
two interested
parties to engage in
agreed upon sexual
activities.
24 and 25 are
exclusionary of
sexual encounters
with more than 2
people.
Comments 21-24 are not
specific on the means by
which the communication for
consent occurs.
25. It's when the
other person clearly
indicates they wish
a sexual act to
happen. This could
be verbal, written
or through body
language (although
24 and 25 are
exclusionary of
sexual encounters
with more than 2
people.
25 is a lacking clarity in their
description.
#25 - I liked the
acknowledgement that body
language is hard to define
because I agree with this.
30
that's a harder line
to define). *
26. A fluid and
reversible assertion
that all members of
a sexual scene are
present and readily
willing to engage in
the acts being
proposed. **
27. Establishing a
clear agreement
that both partners
would like to have
a sexual encounter.
*
28. Sexual consent
is a mutual
agreement between
2 or more people to
engage in sexual
activity - without
coercion or
compensation. All
people must be
capable of
consenting and
agreeing. **
Comment 28 on the next
section mention consent
cannot be obtained if
compensation is
involved. That to me is a false
statement. Compensation can
be a condition of consent and
does not invalidate the
consent. It is not coercion if
the person is seeking
compensation as a condition
of consent.
29. [s]exual consent
is both a cognitive
decision and a
behavioural display
(verbal or
nonverbal),
signaling a
31
willingness (free
from coercion/
incapacitation)
to engage in sexual
activity.***
30. Sexual
Consent is a verbal
or nonverbal
agreement between
two adults to
engage in sexual
acts with one
another. Sexual
consent can be
either clearly
defined or implied
depending on the
setting of the
encounter and
habits of the
individuals. *
31. Sexual consent
is the permission by
another to engage
in a sexual act. The
consent involves
complete choice
from the other
without substance
coercion or force.
**
*Note. Level of agreement * Endorsement = > 50% participant agreement; ** Consensus =
>70% participant agreement *** Major Consensus = >90% participant agreement
32
Of the seven descriptions of sexual consent which the Panel reached a level of consensus
of over 90% (descriptions 3, 4, 5,6,15, 23, & 29), three of the six qualities of sexual consent are
shared by multiple descriptions. The quality Sexual consent should be freely/given without
influence is notable in five of these descriptions (3,4,5,6, 29) and is reflected as a positive
element of these descriptions in the qualitative comments. Indeed, as seen in Table 3 one MSM
participant notes:
I especially agree with the emphasis in 3, 4, 5 about the importance of ongoing
communication and the ability to withdraw consent, as well as the importance of equity and the
role that power dynamics play.
The quality of Sexual consent should be ongoing is documented in in four of the Major
Consensus descriptions (4,5,15, & 23). Again, in Table 3 qualitative feedback reflects the
positive nature of the Major consensus percentage as one SR notes: “4 and 5 include that consent
is an ongoing process, which I see as a key component in the definition”. Lastly, descriptions 23
and 29 shared the sexual consent quality of Sexual consent should be permission granting/
affirming however qualitative feedback for these statements did not endorse that quality as
being relevant to its’ Likert rating.
In Round Two, participants were asked to review the list of six-sexual consent qualities
and provide qualitative feedback speaking to the comprehensiveness of this list as a form of
member check (Appendix AA). Additionally, initial rank order positions of the sexual consent
qualities were collected from participants and are depicted in Table 4. Results from this initial
ranking demonstrate that the group was unable to come to an agreement as no sexual consent
33
quality in a given rank order position was able to reach the minimum tier. However, several
sexual consent qualities in a given rank order position were close to meeting group agreement. In
particular, “Sexual consent should be mutual” reached 40% in the first rank order position (most
important), with seven MSM endorsing it and the first position and one researcher who identified
as MSM placing it in rank order position one. “Consent should be confirmed via verbal and
nonverbal behaviors reached 30% in the sixth rank order position (sixth most important) and
with two SR, four MSM and one MSM identified researcher placing it in the sixth rank order
position.
Table 4,
Round 2, Sexual Consent Qualities Rank Order Position
Table 4
Rank Order Position
1
most
important
position
2
Second
most
important
position
3
Third most
important
position
4
Fourth
most
important
position
5
Fifth most
important
position
6
Sixth most
important
position
Consent
Quality
Round 2
Round 2
Round 2
Round 2
Round 2
Round 2
34
I. Sexual
consent
should be
mutual
between all
parties.
Panel n =8
(40%)
Panel n = 7
(35%)
Panel n = 4
(20%)
Panel n =
1 (5%)
-
-
SR n = 0
SR n = 4
SR n = 1
SR n = 0
MSM n =
8 (1 MSR)
MSM n = 3
MSM n = 3
(1 MSR)
MSM n =
1
II. Sexual
consent
should be
freely/given
without
influence.
Panel n =
4 (20%)
Panel n = 7
(35%)
Panel n =5
(25%)
Panel n =
2 (5%)
Panel n = 1
(5%)
Panel n = 2
(10%)
SR n = 2
SR n = 1
SR n = 1
SR n = 0
SR n = 0
SR n = 1
MSM n =
2
MSM n = 6
(2 MSR)
MSM n = 4
MSM n =
2
MSM n = 1
MSM n = 1
III. Sexual
consent
should be
confirmed
via verbal
and non-
verbal
behaviors.
-
Panel n = 1
(5%)
Panel n = 5
(25%)
Panel n =
3 (15%)
Panel n = 5
(25%)
Panel n = 6
(30%)
SR n = 0
SR n = 2
SR n = 1
SR n = 0
SR n = 2
MSM n = 1
MSM n = 3
MSM n =
2 (1 MSR)
MSM n = 5
MSM n = 5
(1 MSR)
IV. Sexual
consent
Panel n =
1 (5%)
Panel n = 1
(5%)
Panel n = 3
(15%)
Panel n =
6 (30%)
Panel n = 4
(20%)
Panel n =5
(25%)
35
should be
reversible/
revocable.
SR n = 0
SR n = 0
SR n = 0
SR n = 2
SR n = 2
SR n = 1
MSM n =
1
MSM n = 1
MSM n = 3
(1 MSR)
MSM n =
4 (1 MSR)
MSM n = 2
MSM n = 4
V. Sexual
consent
should be
ongoing.
Panel n =
1 (5%)
Panel n = 2
(10%)
-
Panel n =
6 (30%)
Panel n = 9
(45%)
Panel n = 2
(10%)
SR n = 0
SR n = 0
SR n = 2
SR n = 3
SR n = 0
MSM n =
1
MSM n = 2
MSM n =
4
MSM n = 6
(2 MSR)
MSM n = 2
VI. Sexual
consent
should be
permission
granting/
affirming.
Panel n =
6 (30%)
Panel n = 2
(10%)
Panel n = 3
(15%)
Panel n =
3 (15%)
Panel n = 1
(5%)
Panel n =5
(25%)
SR n = 3
SR n = 0
SR n = 1
SR n = 0
SR n = 0
SR n = 1
MSM n =
3 (1 MSR)
MSM n = 2
MSM n = 2
MSM n =
3
MSM n = 1
MSM n = 4
(1 MSR)
Note. When applicable MSM Sexual Researchers are noted within totals as (# MSR)
Data from the initial Round Two ranking was analyzed and collated before being
provided to participants for Round Three. Participants reviewed their initial rank order positions
of sexual consent qualities against the group aggregate, and then re-ranked the sexual consent
qualities. As can be seen Table 4, there are a diverse set of opinions amongst the group. However
some group agreement was reached with a total of 4 sexual consent qualities reaching an
36
agreement level of Endorsement by having 50% or more of the participants agreeing on a rank
order position for the specific quality. In particular, “Sexual consent should be mutual” (first
position n = 11, [61%]), “Sexual consent should be confirmed via verbal and non-verbal
behaviors(third position n = 9, [50%]), “Sexual consent should be ongoing” (fifth position n =
9, [50%]), and Sexual consent should be permission granting/ affirming (sixth position n = 10,
[55%] ).
Table 5,
Round 3, Sexual Consent Qualities Rank Order Position
Table 5
Rank Order Position
1
most
important
position
2
Second
most
important
position
3
Third most
important
position
4
Fourth
most
important
position
5
Fifth most
important
position
6
Sixth most
important
position
Consent
Quality
Round 3
Round 3
Round 3
Round 3
Round 3
Round 3
I. Sexual
consent
should be
mutual
between all
Panel n
=11 (61%)
Panel n = 5
(27%)
Panel n = 2
(11%)
-
-
-
SR n = 3
SR n = 1
SR n = 1
37
parties.
MSM n =
8 (1 MSR)
MSM n = 3
MSM n = 3
(1 MSR)
II. Sexual
consent
should be
freely/given
without
influence.
Panel n =
4 (22%)
Panel n =
10 (55%)
Panel n =2
(11%)
-
Panel n = 2
(11%)
-
SR n = 2
SR n = 3
SR n = 0
SR n = 0
MSM n =
2 (1 MSR)
MSM n = 7
MSM n = 2
MSM n = 2
III. Sexual
consent
should be
confirmed
via verbal
and non-
verbal
behaviors.
Panel n =
1 (5%)
Panel n = 1
(5%)
Panel n = 9
(50%)
Panel n =
3 (16%)
Panel n = 2
(11%)
Panel n = 2
(11%)
SR n = 0
SR n = 1
SR n = 2
SR n = 0
SR n = 2
(1MSR)
SR n = 1
MSM n =
1
MSM n = 0
MSM n = 7
MSM n =
3
MSM n = 0
MSM n = 1
IV. Sexual
consent
should be
reversible/
revocable.
Panel n =
1 (5%)
Panel n = 4
(22%)
Panel n =
7 (38%)
Panel n = 4
(22%)
Panel n =2
(11%)
SR n = 0
SR n = 1
SR n = 3
SR n = 1
SR n = 0
MSM n =
1
MSM n = 3
(1 MSR)
MSM n =
4
MSM n = 3
MSM n = 2
38
V. Sexual
consent
should be
ongoing.
Panel n =
1 (5%)
Panel n = 1
(10%)
-
Panel n =
3 (16%)
Panel n = 9
(50%)
Panel n = 4
(10%)
SR n = 0
SR n = 0
SR n = 2
SR n = 3
SR n = 0
MSM n =
1
MSM n = 1
MSM n =
1
MSM n = 6
MSM n = 4
(1 MSR)
VI. Sexual
consent
should be
permission
granting/
affirming.
Panel n = 1
(5%)
Panel n = 1
(5%)
Panel n =
5 (27%)
Panel n = 1
(5%)
Panel n
=10 (55%)
SR n = 0
SR n = 1
SR n = 0
SR n = 0
SR n = 4
MSM n = 1
MSM n = 0
MSM n =
5 (1 MSR)
MSM n = 1
MSM n = 6
Note. When applicable MSM Sexual Researchers are noted within totals as (# MSR)
Finally, Table 6 compares the first order position rankings of sexual consent qualities
between Round Two and Round Three. As noted in Table six, more agreement was reached
when considering the first rank order position. Sexual consent should be mutual earned a
group endorsement with 61% (n=11) of participants indicating this quality belonged in the first
rank order position. Similarly, Table 7 compares the sixth rank order position of sexual consent
qualities between rounds. The quality of Sexual consent should be permission granting/
affirming also earned an endorsement in the sixth rank order position with 55% of participants
indicating its placement there.
39
Table 6
Sexual Consent Qualities First Rank Order Position Comparison between Round Two Ranking
and Round Three Ranking,
Table 6
Consent Quality
Panel Description
Round 2
First rank
order
position
Round 3
First rank order
position
I. Sexual consent should be
mutual between all parties.
Consensus %
40%
n = 8
61%
n = 11
SR
n = 0
n = 3
MSM
n = 8 (MSR)
n = 8
II. Sexual consent should be
freely/given without influence.
Consensus %
20%
n = 4
22%
n = 4
SR
n = 2
n = 2
MSM
n = 2
n = 2 (1 MSR)
40
III. Sexual consent should be
confirmed via verbal and non-
verbal behaviors.
Consensus %
-
5%
n = 1
SR
-
-
MSM
-
n =1
IV. Sexual consent should be
reversible/ revocable.
Consensus %
5%
(n = 1)
5%
(n = 1)
SR
-
-
MSM
n = 1
n = 1
V. Sexual consent should be
ongoing.
Consensus %
5%
(n = 1)
5%
(n = 1)
SR
-
-
MSM
n = 1
n = 1
VI. Sexual consent should be
permission granting/ affirming.
Consensus %
30%
(n = 6)
-
SR
n = 3
MSM
n = 3 (1
41
MSR)
Note. When applicable MSM Sexual Researchers are noted within totals as (# MSR)
42
Table 7
Sexual Consent Qualities Sixth Rank Order Position Comparison between Round Two Ranking
and Round Three Ranking,
Table 7
Consent Quality
Round 2
Sixth rank
order
position
Round 3
Sixth rank
order position
I. Sexual consent should be
mutual between all parties.
Consensus %
-
-
SR
-
-
MSM
-
-
II. Sexual consent should be
freely/given without influence.
Consensus %
10% (n = 2)
-
SR
n = 1
-
MSM
n = 1
-
III. Sexual consent should be
Consensus %
30% (n = 6)
11% (n = 2)
43
Consent Quality
Round 2
Sixth rank
order
position
Round 3
Sixth rank
order position
confirmed via verbal and non-
verbal behaviors.
SR
n = 2
n = 1
MSM
n = 5 (1
MSR)
n = 1
IV. Sexual consent should be
reversible/ revocable.
Consensus %
25% (n = 5)
5% (n = 1)
SR
n = 1
-
MSM
n = 4
n = 1
V. Sexual consent should be
ongoing.
Consensus %
5% (n = 1)
11% (n = 2)
SR
-
-
MSM
n = 1
n = 2
VI. Sexual consent should be
permission granting/ affirming.
Consensus %
25% (n = 5)
55% (n = 10)
SR
n = 1
n = 4
44
Consent Quality
Round 2
Sixth rank
order
position
Round 3
Sixth rank
order position
MSM
n = 4 (1
MSR)
n = 6
Note. When applicable MSM Sexual Researchers are noted within totals as (# MSR)
Sexual Non-Consent Results
Questions regarding Sexual non-consent where not added into the survey until Round
Two due to a Qualtrics error. In Round Two, participants were asked to respond to the open-
ended prompt of “How would you describe sexual non-consent?” participants provided 20
unique qualitative descriptions of sexual non-consent (See left panel of Table 8). The 20
descriptions of sexual non-consent were analyzed using open and axial coding and formed the
five elements of sexual non-consent shown below:
1. Non-consent involves Coercion and Power Imbalances that prevent consent to be freely
given (by at least one member of the party).
2. Non-consent involves a lack of Mutual Agreement (between all members of the party)
3. Non-consent involves a lack of Will or Desire (for at least one member of the party)
4. Non-consent involves Verbal, Nonverbal, and Body Language behaviors to communicate
it.
5. Non-consent involves the continued violation of permission (of at least one party
45
member) as conveyed through verbal behaviors (eg. saying no).
Non-Consent element one was derived from the broad category of (1) Coercive/ power
Imbalance. Descriptions coded with Coercive/ Power imbalance were descriptions in which
the participant noted the implications of unequal power dynamics, citing a variety of origins
for that imbalance (e.g., social, perceived, chronological, neurological, etc.). Five responses
were coded with the Coercive/ Power imbalance category, an example of these statements is:
“Non-consent is any sexual action that occurs that is not agreed upon, occurs with a power
differential, one of the members is influenced by a substance, there is force, or finally that
consent is withdrawn but the action continues”.
Broad Category (2) Lack of Mutual Agreement was utilized when a description
emphasized a one-sidedness or lack of agreement as being part of sexual non-consent. This
code was utilized six times during the coding process. An exemplar statement for this code is
“Sexual non-consent is the act of forcing or coercing an individual into sex without his/her
permission”.
Broad category (3) Lack of Will/ Desire was used to code descriptions in which the
participant identified lack of will/ desire as part of sexual non-consent. This code was applied
ten times and is represented be the following statement: “Choosing to not want to participate
in sexual activity. This can occur independently of a provocation or can happen by declining
a suggestion or proposition. Non-consent means no.”
The next Broad category (4) Nonverbals / Body language was derived from participants
response which noted the use of nonverbals or “body language” to effectively convey a lack
of consent to a partner. This code is best represented by the following description:
“Either actively dissenting (verbally by saying "No" or otherwise indicating distaste,
46
disinterest; nonverbally by physically resisting or moving away from person seeking consent)
AND passively resisting (maybe verbally saying things like, "Maybe" or "I'm just really
tired"; nonverbally lying motionless or not moving). Basically, any verbal language that is
not "Yes" but "Maybe" or "No" is a no to me, and any body language that is ambiguous or
disinterested is non-consent, in my book.”
This code was used a total of ten times during the analysis.
The final Broad category (5) Violation of Permission was derived from descriptions use
of terminology in which boundaries are communicated (via verbal or nonverbal behaviors) and
permission has been denied to proceed further in activity by one party member. This code was
utilized a total of 8 times during the analysis. Violation of Permission is best exemplified by this
statement: “Any sexual experience that leaves any partner questioning their own enthusiastic and
willing participation or feeling that boundaries and limits were ignored through verbal, non-
verbal, or socially coercive actions or words.”
In Round Three, the 20 original descriptions of sexual non-consent (collected in Round
Two) were reviewed by participants and rated on a five-point Likert scale with 1 being “This
does not adequately describe sexual non-consent” and 5 “This very much describes sexual non-
consent”. Table 6 includes descriptions of sexual non-consent provided by respondents as well
as their overall rating on the five-point Likert scale. Of the 20 descriptions of sexual non-consent
rated in round two, 19 descriptions (95%) met a tier of group agreement. One description,
(description 10) met consensus negatively receiving 60% of participants endorsing this as not
adequately describing non consent. Reviewing the remaining 18 descriptions, one response
(description 7) met Major Consensus with 94% of respondents indicating a four or five on the
Likert scale; nine descriptions established consensus with at least 70% or participants endorsing
47
a 4 or 5 on the Likert scale; and 8 descriptions of sexual non-consent, marked a group
Endorsement with a minimum of 50% of participants indicating a four or five on the Likert scale.
Table 8 also shows means and standard deviations for the entire panel of participants, as well as
participants broken down into stakeholder status.
48
Table 8
Round 3, Task 1, Level of Agreement to Descriptions of Sexual non -consent 1(Strongly
Disagree) to 5 (Strongly Agree) with Mean, Standard Deviation, and Variance
49
Description
M
SD
Consensus %
1. Any sexual experience that leaves any partner
questioning their own enthusiastic and willing
participation or feeling that boundaries and
limits were ignored through verbal, non-verbal,
or socially coercive actions or words. **
Panel
4.22
.87
83% (n = 15)
SR
4.2
.40
100% (n = 5)
MSM
4.33
.98
83% (n = 10)
MSR
3.0
0
-
2. Non-consent is the way that two or more people
say no leading up to and during a sexual
encounter.
Panel
3.5
1.1
44% (n = 8)
SR
3.2
.97
20% (n = 1)
MSM
3.5
1.1
50% (n = 6)
MSR
5.0
0
100% (n = 1)
3. Verbal expression of non-consent, body
language such as moving away from the other
person(s), physical deflection of a sexual
contact, or non verbal queue such as shaking of
the head from side to side. **
Panel
4.1
1.1
82% (n = 14)
SR
3.6
.48
60% (n = 3)
MSM
4.1
.93
83% (n = 10)
50
MSR
5.0
0.0
100% (n = 1)
4. Reluctance or resistance to a suggestion of
sexual activity. It can be verbally, physically or
in body language. **
Panel
4.1
1.1
72% (n = 13)
SR
3.8
.97
60% (n = 3)
MSM
4.1
1.1
75% (n = 9)
MSR
5.0
0.0
100% (n = 1)
5. Rape*
Panel
4
1.4
66% (n=12)
SR
3.4
1.4
40 % (n = 2)
MSM
4.1
1.3
75% (n = 9)
MSR
5.0
0.0
100% (n = 1)
6. denial of permission, verbally or non-verbally
**
Panel
3.9
1.2
72% (n=13)
SR
3.6
1.2
40% (n=2)
MSM
4.0
1.3
83% (n = 10)
MSR
5.0
0.0
100% (n = 1)
51
7. Not giving consent would include when not all
parties agree to the sexual activity. This non-
consent can take the form of verbal and non-
verbal cues (e.g., saying "No," pushing
someone away), reversing a decision for
consent that may have been given earlier, when
an involved party is not an adult, or an adult in
a power dynamic not capable of consent in the
first place (e.g. child, prisoner, mental health
considerations). ***
Panel
4.7
.57
94% (n=17)
SR
4.6
.48
100% (n = 5)
MSM
4.7
.621
91% (n = 11)
MSR
5.0
0.0
100% (n = 1)
8. Verbal or non-verbal indications that they are
unwilling or not wanting to participate in sexual
activities. Any Coercive or being under the
influence of drugs or alcohol automatically
makes it non-consensual. **
Panel
3.8
.96
72% (n = 13)
SR
3.6
.48
60% (n = 3)
MSM
4.0
1.12
75% (n = 9)
MSR
4.0
0.0
100% (n = 1)
9. I would describe sexual non-consent as taking
actions to indicate that you do not want to have
sex in this moment. Preferably it involves
words indicating your unwillingness to engage
in sex, but it can also involve non-verbal
behaviors such as increasing physical
distance.*
Panel
4.0
1.1
66% (n = 12)
SR
3.4
.48
60 % (n = 3)
MSM
4.1
.93
66% (n = 8)
MSR
5.0
0.0
100% (n = 1)
10. If the other party utterly do not agree
Panel
2.8
1.3
27% (n=5)
52
SR
1.8
1.1
20% (n = 1)
MSM
3.0
1.1
25% (n =3)
MSR
5.0
0.0
100% (n = 1)
11. Non-consent is any sexual action that occurs
that is not agreed upon, occurs with a power
differential, one of the members is influenced
by a substance, there is force, or finally that
consent is withdrawn but the action continues.
**
Panel
4.2
1.2
77% (n = 14)
SR
4
1.0
80% (n = 4)
MSM
4.3
1.2
75% (n = 9)
MSR
5.0
0.0
100% (n = 1)
12. Either actively dissenting (verbally by saying
"No" or otherwise indicating distaste,
disinterest; nonverbally by physically resisting
or moving away from person seeking consent)
AND passively resisting (maybe verbally
saying things like, "Maybe" or "I'm just really
tired"; nonverbally lying motionless or not
moving). Basically, any verbal language that is
not "Yes" but "Maybe" or "No" is a no to me,
and any body language that is ambiguous or
disinterested is non-consent, in my book. **
Panel
4.2
.78
77% (n=14)
SR
4.2
.74
80% (n = 4)
MSM
4.1
.79
75% (n = 9)
MSR
5.0
0.0
100% (n = 1)
13. Sexual non-consent is the act of forcing or
coercing an individual into sex without his/her
permission.*
Panel
3.5
1.3
55% (n=10)
SR
3.4
.8
40% (n = 2)
53
MSM
3.6
1.4
58% (n = 7)
MSR
5.0
0.0
100% (n = 1)
14. Sexual non consent is an unwillingness to
participate in a sexual act with one or more
other people. This can be affirmed actively or
through avoidance. Sexual non consent should
be the default assumption until confirmed.*
Panel
3.6
1.0
55% (n = 10)
SR
3.4
.80
40% (n = 2)
MSM
3.6
1.1
58% (n = 7)
MSR
5.0
0.0
100% (n = 1)
15. Not wishing to engage in certain sexual
activities with a certain partner or partners, or at
that specific time. This can also include
moments where an individual is unable to
provide consent, such as when someone is
drinking alcohol or is unconscious.*
Panel
3.8
1.3
61% (n=11)
SR
4
1.1
60% (n = 3)
MSM
3.8
1.4
58% (n = 7)
MSR
5.0
0.0
100% (n = 1)
16. Non-consent constitutes the absence of
agreement. In other words, a lack of
disagreement or refusal is not sufficient for
consent. Sexual non-consent is sexual activity
that is not mutual, communicated
(verbal/nonverbal), or ongoing.**
Panel
4.3
1.3
83% (n=15)
SR
4.6
.8
80% (n = 4)
MSM
4.1
1.1
83% (n = 10)
54
MSR
5.0
0.0
100% (n = 1)
17. Saying no. Or some concept of that. Actions
too, but honestly, I think no is very clear.*
Panel
3.3
1.4
55% (n=10)
SR
2.2
1.5
40% (n = 2)
MSM
3.5
1.1
58% (n = 7)
MSR
5.0
0.0
100% (n = 1)
18. Choosing to not want to participate in sexual
activity. This can occur independently of a
provocation, or can happen by declining a
suggestion or proposition. Non-consent means
no.*
Panel
3.6
1.2
61% (n=11)
SR
2.8
1.2
40% (n = 2)
MSM
3.8
1.1
66% (n = 8)
MSR
5.0
0.0
100% (n = 1)
19. Non-consent is either verbal or nonverbal
communication that states someone in the party
does not want to participate. When someone is
non-consenting the act should be stopped
immediately and not face any harm or backlash.
**
Panel
4.0
1.2
77% (n = 14)
SR
3.6
1.35
80% (n = 4)
MSM
4.1
1.2
83% (n = 10)
MSR
5.0
0.0
100% (n = 1)
55
20. The forcing of a sexual activity or encounter on
someone who has not given clear consent or the
continuation of sexual acts on someone who
has rescinded their consent.*
Panel
3.8
.92
66% (n=13)
SR
3.2
.97
40% (n = 2)
MSM
4.0
.74
83% (n = 10)
MSR
5.0
0.0
100% (n = 1)
*Note. Level of agreement * Endorsement = > 50% participant agreement ; ** Consensus =
>70% participant agreement *** Major Consensus = >90% participant agreement
Additionally, during Round Three, qualitative feedback was also collected from participants
regarding their reactions to peers’ descriptions of sexual non-consent collected in Round Two.
Table 9 contains the qualitative comments made by participants in response to peers’
descriptions of sexual non-consent. Comments were sorted by description reference number and
according to stakeholder status (SR and MSM). Table 9 notes differences between these groups,
and particularly highlights differing views on substance use and its impact on the ability to give
sexual consent (comments responding to 8 and 11). Additionally, overarching themes through
these comments highlight the role of coercion and power in sexual non-consent (comments 16-
20).
Table 9,
56
Round 3, Task 1 Qualitative Feedback to participants descriptions
Table 8
How would you describe
sexual non-consent?
Sexual Researcher
MSM / MSR
1. Any sexual
experience that
leaves any partner
questioning their
own enthusiastic
and willing
participation or
feeling that
boundaries and
limits were ignored
through verbal,
non-verbal, or
socially coercive
actions or words.
**
2. Non-consent is the
way that two or
more people say no
leading up to and
during a sexual
encounter.
3. Verbal expression
of non-consent,
body language
such as moving
away from the
other person(s),
physical deflection
of a sexual contact,
or non verbal
57
queue such as
shaking of the head
from side to side.
**
4. Reluctance or
resistance to a
suggestion of
sexual activity. It
can be verbally,
physically or in
body language. **
5. Rape*
6. denial of
permission,
verbally or non-
verbally **
Again, 6-10 show nuanced parts of
non-consent. There are many
examples, and I think all these are
valid.
7. Not giving consent
would include
when not all parties
agree to the sexual
activity. This non-
consent can take
the form of verbal
and non-verbal
cues (e.g., saying
"No," pushing
someone away),
reversing a
decision for
consent that may
have been given
earlier, when an
involved party is
not an adult, or an
Again, 6-10 show nuanced parts of
non-consent. There are many
examples, and I think all these are
valid.
No 7 and 8 are great examples because
they also include mental state (clouded
by drugs or alcohol) and power
dynamics as forcing non consent. Non
consent is more than just no
58
adult in a power
dynamic not
capable of consent
in the first place
(e.g. child,
prisoner, mental
health
considerations).***
8. Verbal or non-
verbal indications
that they are
unwilling or not
wanting to
participate in
sexual activities.
Any Coercive or
being under the
influence of drugs
or alcohol
automatically
makes it non-
consensual.**
Comment 8: I do not agree that if a
person is under the influence of alcohol
it is automatically not consent. I have
had sex many times while under the
influence of alcohol and I had the
capacity to provide my consent. There
are degrees of influence or impairment
and that is what must be considered.
Again, 6-10 show nuanced parts of
non-consent. There are many
examples, and I think all these are
valid.
No 7 and 8 are great examples because
they also include mental state (clouded
by drugs or alcohol) and power
dynamics as forcing non consent. Non
consent is more than just no
9. I would describe
sexual non-consent
as taking actions to
indicate that you
do not want to
have sex in this
moment.
Preferably it
involves words
indicating your
unwillingness to
engage in sex, but
it can also involve
non-verbal
Again, 6-10 show nuanced parts of
non-consent. There are many
examples, and I think all these are
valid.
59
behaviors such as
increasing physical
distance.*
10. If the other party
utterly do not agree
Comment 10: This comment provides
no description of how any party is not
agreeing and is inadequate.
No 10 is too vague. How do you know
if someone else doesn't agree?
10 seems to be lacking description and
clearness in language.
Again, 6-10 show nuanced parts of
non-consent. There are many
examples, and I think all these are
valid.
11. Non-consent is any
sexual action that
occurs that is not
agreed upon,
occurs with a
power differential,
one of the
members is
influenced by a
substance, there is
force, or finally
that consent is
withdrawn but the
action continues.**
11 and 13 both seem
to be more about the
non consensual act
perpetrated, not non-
consent given (or not
given) by the other
party
Comments 11 and 15: The fact that a
person has consumed alcohol does not
automatically mean they are unable to
provide consent. Again the degree to
which alcohol is impacting the
individual's ability to make a decision
must be considered.
No 11 is a particularly good non
consent definition. It includes all
dynamics I can think of (coercion,
mental state, force, unwillingness).
This is probably the best definition I
can find.
60
12. Either actively
dissenting
(verbally by saying
"No" or otherwise
indicating distaste,
disinterest;
nonverbally by
physically resisting
or moving away
from person
seeking consent)
AND passively
resisting (maybe
verbally saying
things like,
"Maybe" or "I'm
just really tired";
nonverbally lying
motionless or not
moving). Basically,
any verbal
language that is not
"Yes" but "Maybe"
or "No" is a no to
me, and any body
language that is
ambiguous or
disinterested is
non-consent, in my
book.**
13. Sexual non-consent
is the act of forcing
or coercing an
individual into sex
without his/her
permission.*
11 and 13 both seem
to be more about the
non consensual act
perpetrated, not non-
consent given (or not
given) by the other
party
Comment 13 describes one forcing a
sexual act, not the act of a the person
giving non-consent.
No 13 is too vague and doesn't capture
the range of different non consent
conditions.
61
14. Sexual non consent
is an unwillingness
to participate in a
sexual act with one
or more other
people. This can be
affirmed actively
or through
avoidance. Sexual
non consent should
be the default
assumption until
confirmed.*
Comment 14: Does not describe any
ways specific ways that non-consent is
conveyed.
#14 gets to an important approach, that
non-consent should be the default until
consent is confirmed. I hadn't thought
of that before in that way, it makes
sense.
15. Not wishing to
engage in certain
sexual activities
with a certain
partner or partners,
or at that specific
time. This can also
include moments
where an
individual is
unable to provide
consent, such as
when someone is
drinking alcohol or
is unconscious.*
Comments 11 and 15: The fact that a
person has consumed alcohol does not
automatically mean they are unable to
provide consent. Again the degree to
which alcohol is impacting the
individual's ability to make a decision
must be considered.
16. Non-consent
constitutes the
absence of
agreement. In other
words, a lack of
disagreement or
refusal is not
sufficient for
consent. Sexual
non-consent is
sexual activity that
is not mutual,
No 16 through 20 are all pretty good,
but still focus only on not declining
and not the other attributes of consent
(metnal state, coercion, ability to
consent).
62
communicated
(verbal/nonverbal),
or ongoing.**
17. Saying no. Or
some concept of
that. Actions too,
but honestly, I
think no is very
clear.*
No 16 through 20 are all pretty good,
but still focus only on not declining
and not the other attributes of consent
(metnal state, coercion, ability to
consent).
18. Choosing to not
want to participate
in sexual activity.
This can occur
independently of a
provocation, or can
happen by
declining a
suggestion or
proposition. Non-
consent means no.*
No 16 through 20 are all pretty good,
but still focus only on not declining
and not the other attributes of consent
(metnal state, coercion, ability to
consent).
19. Non-consent is
either verbal or
nonverbal
communication
that states someone
in the party does
not want to
participate. When
someone is non-
consenting the act
should be stopped
immediately and
not face any harm
or backlash.**
No 16 through 20 are all pretty good,
but still focus only on not declining
and not the other attributes of consent
(metnal state, coercion, ability to
consent).
#19 is mine! I remember. haha I think I
might have forgot last time. I really
liked the "no is very clear" because I
agree, it should be very clear. But I
think our culture doesn't allow for that.
There is always an unbalance of power
and people will exploit it if they know
they can get away with it. It's terrible.
63
20. The forcing of a
sexual activity or
encounter on
someone who has
not given clear
consent or the
continuation of
sexual acts on
someone who has
rescinded their
consent.*
same criticism for
#20 as for 11 and 13
- more about the
more active party
than the person not
consenting
No 16 through 20 are all pretty good,
but still focus only on not declining
and not the other attributes of consent
(metnal state, coercion, ability to
consent).
Referencing 16-20
Still a lil [sic] mind
boggled coming up
with a strong
definition of the
absence of
something.
*Note. Level of agreement * Endorsement = > 50% participant agreement ; ** Consensus =
>70% participant agreement *** Major Consensus = >90% participant agreement
After reviewing peers' descriptions of sexual non-consent, participants were given the five
elements of sexual non-consent derived from the group’s descriptions of sexual non-consent and
asked to provide feedback. Furthermore, participants were asked to rank order the elements with
rank order position one being “most impactful” on a 1-5 scale. Data collected from this ranking
is depicted below in Table 10. Only one element of non-consent Non-consent involves a lack of
Will or Desire (for at least one member of the party)reached a level of agreement by
participants, an endorsement (50%, n=9), and this was placed in the fifth rank order position.
Two other responses neared a group agreement of an endorsement, “Non-consent involves a lack
of Mutual Agreement (between all members of the party) in the first rank order position (44%,
64
n=8) and “Non-consent involves Coercion and Power Imbalances that prevent consent to be
freely given (by at least one member of the party)” in the third rank order position (44% n=8).
Table 10
Elements of Sexual Non-Consent Rank Order Position Round 3
Table 9
Rank Order Position
1
most
impactful
position
2
Second
most
impactful
position
3
Third most
impactful
position
4
Fourth
most
impactful
position
5
Fifth most
impactful
position
Non-
Consent
Element
Round 3
Round 3
Round 3
Round 3
Round 3
I. Non-
consent
involves
Coercion
and Power
Imbalances
that prevent
consent to
be freely
given (by at
least one
member of
Panel n =
2 (11%)
Panel n = 3
(16%)
Panel n = 8
(44%)
Panel n =
3 (16%)
Panel n = 2
(11%)
SR n = 1
SR n = 0
SR n = 2
SR n = 2
SR n = 0
MSM n =
1
MSM n = 3
MSM n = 6
MSM n =
1 (1 MSR)
MSM n = 2
65
the party).
II. Non-
consent
involves a
lack of
Mutual
Agreement
(between all
members of
the party)
Panel n =
8 (44%)
Panel n = 2
(11%)
Panel n =2
(11%)
Panel n =
5 (27%)
Panel n = 1
(6%)
SR n = 3
SR n = 0
SR n = 2
SR n = 0
SR n = 0
MSM n =
5
MSM n = 2
MSM n = 0
MSM n =
5 (1 MSR)
MSM n = 1
III. Non-
consent
involves a
lack of Will
or Desire
(for at least
one member
of the
party).
Panel n =
1 (6%)
Panel n = 4
(22%)
Panel n = 2
(11%)
Panel n =
2 (11%)
Panel n = 9
(50%)
SR n = 0
SR n = 2
SR n = 0
SR n = 1
SR n = 2
MSM n =
1
MSM n = 2
MSM n = 2
MSM n =
1
MSM n = 6
IV. Non-
consent
involves
Verbal,
Nonverbal,
and Body
Language
behaviors to
communicat
e it.
Panel n =
4 (22%)
Panel n = 6
(33%)
Panel n = 4
(22%)
Panel n =
3 (16%)
Panel n = 1
62%)
SR n = 1
SR n = 2
SR n = 1
SR n = 0
SR n = 1
MSM n =
3
MSM n = 3
(1 MSR)
MSM n = 3
MSM n =
3
MSM n = 0
66
V. Non-
consent
involves the
continued
violation of
permission
(of at least
one party
member) as
conveyed
through
verbal
behaviors
(eg. saying
no).
Panel n =
3 (16 %)
Panel n = 4
(16%)
Panel n = 2
(11%)
Panel n =
5 (27%)
Panel n = 5
(27%)
SR n = 0
SR n = 1
SR n = 0
SR n = 2
SR n = 2
MSM n =
3 (1 MSR)
MSM n = 3
MSM n = 2
MSM n =
3
MSM n = 3
Note. When applicable MSM Sexual Researchers are noted within totals as (# MSR)
Nonverbal Sexual Consent Communication Behaviors Results
Across Rounds One and Three, data was collected from participants regarding nonverbal
sexual consent communication behaviors. In Round One, participants reviewed Beres et al.’s
(2007) list of nonverbal sexual consent communication behaviors and provided qualitative
feedback regarding the list (Table 11). Broad themes amongst the feedback regarding nonverbal
consent communication behaviors included the need for “nodding and smiling” and more
attention to cruising behaviors. In addition, as highlighted by one sexual researcher’s comment
“… [You] Need to keep in mind actions that (a) are used to initiate sex that are signaling one’s
own consent and (b) cues that are used in response to someone else’s actions that signal one’s
own consent. Utilizing the feedback collected in Round One, an expanded list of nonverbal
sexual consent communication behaviors was created and presented to participants in Round
Three.
67
Table 11
Round One, Qualitative descriptions of missing nonverbal sexual consent communication
behaviors.
Table 10
Question 61
SR (n= 13)
MSM (n=22)
“When describing the idea of
sexual consent, researchers
have established two means of
communicating consent
through verbal and nonverbal
behaviors. Some of these
nonverbal behaviors have been
noted below (Beres, Herold,
Maitland, 2004).
What nonverbal behaviors, if
any, are missing from the
above list?
None (n=10, 76%)
None (n=9, 40%)
I wanna say like
grinding on them but
maybe that’s what they
mean by “physically
close to your partner,”
haha. Maybe moving
towards bedroom?
Heavy breathing, motions or
gestures that indicate a
request to engage in sexual
activity or mimic sexual
activity
Nodding. Smiling.
Hold partner's hands during
sexual act
There are a couple of
items here noting
“kissing” in return (i.e.,
as a response) but not as
an initial action. Need to
keep in mind actions
that (a) are used to
initiate sex that are
signaling one’s own
consent and (b) cues that
are used in response to
someone else’s actions
that signal one’s own
consent
Head nod in agreement or as
to motion to come
closer/approach. Pass a
prospective partner several
times with eye contact,
"cruising". Gesturing with
hand(s) to approach. Sit or
stand next to deliberately
[sic] leading to touching,
kissing, fondling, etc.
Leaving a shower
curtain/door open at home or
in public place such as gym.
Flashing head lights of a car.
Standing at a urinal for an
68
unusually long time. Many
of these and those listed in
the question are used in
combination to convey
consent.
Partner nods head. You
exchange a "look" that you
know means "yes" to sexual
activity
nod, wink, come hither
motion
Urging on, being
enthusiastic about it
Undressing yourself in front
of your partner, showing off
attractive underwear to your
partner, putting your partners
hands on you
play footsie with your
partner
Raising Eyebrows and eye
contact with the penis.
I would say eye contact and
maybe a head nod yes or no.
I always think verbal is
much more important than
the nonverbals.
Nodding head affirmatively.
Smiling. Making noises, like
moaning.
Sending suggestive or
sexually explicit pictures /
video; Checking out partner
by looking at their body
I want us together
Note. When applicable MSM Sexual Researchers are noted within totals as (# MSR)
69
In Round Three, participants sorted the expanded list of 29 nonverbal sexual consent
behaviors into categories of usage (i.e., identified a behavior as a Consent Giving Behavior, a
Consent Seeking Behavior, an Interchangeable Behavior, Ambiguous Behavior, a Refusal
Behavior or an Unused Behavior). Table12, depicts the results from this sorting task with 24
nonverbal behaviors (82%) reaching a level of group agreement on its usage. Out of the 24
nonverbal sexual consent communication behaviors which reached a level of group agreement,
only The behavior of distancing onself physically from a partner reached the level of Major
Consensus with 95% of the panel labeling this behavior as a consent refusal behavior. Five
behaviors (20%) reached the level of Consensus with more than 70% of the panel agreeing on
the nonverbal communication behavior’s usage (Table 13). The remaining 18 behaviors reached
the level of group endorsement with more than 50% of the panel agreeing on the nonverbal
sexual consent communication behavior’s usage (Table 14). Additionally, a chi square analysis
of independence was run to analyze group differences between SR and MSM (including MSR).
One Behavior, “The behavior of saying nothing and proceeding to have sex with a partner
reported a value of X
2
(3, N= 18) = 6.70, p .08 nearing significance but not meeting threshold for
independence. When examining three distinct groups, SR, MSM, and MSR independently, four
behaviors (11, 15, 21, 23 bolded in Table 12) were found to have p values <.05 suggesting these
groups may act differently than differences between groups. Implications of these findings are
discussed in the next section (crosstabs located in Appendix A).
Table 12,
Round Three, Nonverbal Sexual Consent Communication Behavior Usage
70
Table 11
Sorted Category
Nonverbal
Behavior
Consent
Giving
Consent
Seeking
Interchang
eable
Behavior
Ambiguou
s Behavior
Consent
Refusal
Decline
d to sort
1. The
behavior
of hugging
and
caressing a
partner.
(NV) *
Panel
N = 1
(5%)
N = 2
(11%)
N = 10
(55%)*
N = 4
(22%)
0
N = 1
(5%)
SR
-
-
N = 5
(100%)
-
-
-
MSM
N = 1
(8%)
N = 2
(16%)
N = 5
(41%)
N = 4
(33%)
-
N = 1
(8%)
(1
MSR)
2. The
behavior
of getting
physically
closer to a
partner.
(NV) *
Panel
N = 1
(5%)
N = 5
(27%)
N = 9
(50%)*
N = 3
(16%)
0
0
SR
-
N = 1
(20%)
N = 3
(60%)
N = 1
(20%)
-
-
MSM
N = 1
(8%)
N = 4
(33%)
(1 MSR)
N = 6
(46%)
N = 2
(16%)
-
-
3. The
behavior
of
distancing
onself
physically
from a
partner
(NV) ***
Panel
0
0
0
N = 1
(5%)
N = 17
(95%)*
**
0
SR
-
-
-
-
N = 5
(100%)
-
MSM
-
-
-
N = 1
(8%)
N = 12
(91%)
(1MSR)
-
4. The
behavior
of
touching
and kissing
a partner in
return.
Panel
N = 14
(77%)**
0
N= 4
(22%)
0
0
0
SR
N = 5
(100%)
-
-
-
-
-
MSM
N = 9
(66%)
-
N=4
(33%)
-
-
-
71
(NV)**
(1MSR)
5. The
behavior
of one
beginning
to touch
and kiss a
partner
(NV) **
Panel
0
N = 14
(77%)**
N = 3
(16%)
0
0
N = 1
(5%)
SR
-
N = 3
(60%)
N = 2
(40%)
-
-
-
MSM
-
N = 11
(83%)
(1 MSR)
N = 1
(8%)
-
-
N = 1
(8%)
6. The
behavior
of smiling
at a
partner.
(NV) *
Panel
0
N = 1
(5%)
N = 6
(33%)
N = 9
(50%)*
0.00
N = 2
(11%)
SR
-
-
N = 2
(40%)
N = 3
(60%)
-
-
MSM
-
N = 1
(8%)
N=4
(33%)
N = 6
(50%)
-
N = 2
(8%)
(1
MSR)
7. The
behavior
of not
smiling at
a partner
(NV)*
Panel
0
0
N = 1
(5%)
N= 12
(66%)*
N= 5
(27%)
0
SR
-
-
-
N = 5
(100%)
-
-
MSM
-
-
N = 1
(8%)
N = 7
(58%)
N=5
(33%)
(1
MSR)
-
8. The
behavior
of rubbing,
fondling,
and
touching a
partner
sexually.
(NV)
Panel
N = 1
(5%)
N = 6
(33%)
N = 8
(44%)
N = 1
(5%)
0
N = 2
(11%)
SR
-
N =1
(20%)
N = 3
(60%)
N =1
(20%)
-
-
MSM
N = 1
(8%)
N = 5
(41%)
N = 5
(41%)
-
-
N = 12
(8%) (1
MSR)
9. The
Panel
N = 3
0
N = 2
N= 10
N = 2
N = 1
72
behavior
of not
resisting a
partner’s
sexual
advances.
(NV)*
(16%)
(11%)
(55%)*
(11%)
(5%)
SR
N =1
(20%)
-
-
N = 4
(80%)
-
-
MSM
N = 2
(16%)
-
N = 2
(16%)
N = 6
(41%) ( 1
MSR)
N = 2
(16%)
N = 1
(8%)
10. The
behavior of
not stopping
a partner
from kissing
or touching
one sexually.
(NV)*
Panel
N = 6
(33%)
N = 1
(5%)
0
N= 9
(50%)*
N = 1
(5%)
N = 1
(5%)
SR
N =1
(20%)
-
-
N = 4
(80%)
-
-
MSM
N = 5
(41%)
N = 1
(8%)
-
N=5
(33%) (1
MSR)
N = 1
(8%)
N = 1
(8%)
11.The
behavior of
letting a
partner take
one’s clothes
off. (NV)*
Panel
N=9
(50%)*
N = 1
(5%)
N = 3
(16%)
N= 4
(22%)
0
N = 1
(5%)
SR
N = 2
(40%)
-
N =1
(20%)
N = 2
(40%)
-
-
MSM
N = 7
(58%)
N = 1
(8%)
N = 2
(16%)
N = 2
(16%)
-
N= 1 (1
MSR)
12.The
behavior of
not saying
“no” to a
partner. (NV)
Panel
N = 3
(16%)
0
N = 1
(5%)
N=8
(38%)
N=5
(27%)
N = 1
(5%)
SR
-
-
-
N = 2
(40%)
N = 3
(60%)
-
MSM
N = 3
(24%)
-
N = 1
(8%)
N = 6
(41%) (1
MSR)
N = 2
(16%)
N = 1
(8%)
13.The
behavior of
undressing a
partner.
(NV)*
Panel
0
N=9
(50%)*
N= 4
(22%)
N = 3
(16%)
0
N = 2
(11%)
SR
-
N = 3
(60%)
N =1
(20%)
N =1
(20%)
-
-
MSM
-
N = 6
(50%)
N = 3
(24%)
N = 2
(16%)
-
N = 2
(8%) 1
(MSR)
73
14. The
behavior of
undressing
onself (NV) *
Panel
N = 3
(16%)
N = 3
(16%)
N = 11
(61%)*
0
N = 1
(5%)
0
SR
N =1
(20%)
-
N = 4
(80%)
-
-
-
MSM
N = 2
(16%)
N = 3
(24%)
N = 7
(50%)
-
N = 1
(8%)
-
15. The
behavior of
helping a
partner
undress
oneself. (NV)
Panel
N=5
(27%)
N= 4
(22%)
N = 7
(38%)
N = 1
(5%)
0
N = 1
(5%)
SR
N = 3
(60%)
-
N = 2
(40%)
-
-
-
MSM
N = 2
(16%)
N=4
(33%)
N = 5
(41%)
N = 1
(8%)
-
N = 1 ()
(1
MSR)
16. The
behavior of
helping
undress a
partner. (NV)
Panel
N= 4
(22%)
N = 6
(33%)
N = 7
(38%)
0
0
N = 1
(5%)
SR
N = 2
(40%)
N =1
(20%)
N = 2
(40%)
-
-
-
MSM
N = 2
(16%)
N=5
(33%) (
1 MSR)
N = 5
(41%)
-
-
N = 1
(8%)
17. The
behavior of
following a
partner when
invited.
(NV)*
Panel
N=12
(66%)*
N = 2
(11%)
0
N = 4
(22%)
0
0
SR
N = 4
(80%)
-
-
N =1
(20%)
-
-
MSM
N = 8
(58%) (
1 MSR)
N = 2
(16%)
-
N = 3
(24%)
-
-
18. The
behavior of
signaling or
motioning for
a partner to
follow (NV)
*
Panel
N = 2
(11%)
N=9
(50%)*
N = 2
(11%)
N = 5
(27%)
0
0
SR
N =1
(20%)
N = 2
(40%)
N =1
(20%)
N =1
(20%)
-
-
MSM
N = 1
(8%)
N = 7
(50%) (1
MSR)
N = 1
(8%)
N=4
(33%)
-
-
74
19. The
behavior of
putting one’s
hands down a
partner’s
pants. (NV) *
Panel
0.00
N = 10
(55%)*
N = 5
(27%)
N = 2
(11%)
0
N = 1
(5%)
SR
-
N =1
(20%)
N = 3
(60%)
N =1
(20%)
-
-
MSM
-
N = 9
(66%)
(1 MSR)
N = 2
(16%)
N = 1
(8%)
-
N = 1
(8%)
20. The
behavior of
saying
nothing and
proceeding to
have sex with
a partner.
(NV) *
Panel
N = 4
(22%)
0
N = 3
(16%)
N =11
(61%)*
0
0
SR
-
-
N = 2
(40%)
N = 3
(60%)
-
-
MSM
N = 4
(33%)
-
N = 1
(8%)
N = 8
(58%) (1
MSR)
-
-
21. The
behavior of
touching
oneself. (NV)
*
Panel
0
N= 4
(22%)
N = 10
(55%)*
N = 2
(11%)
0
N = 2
(11%)
SR
-
-
N = 5
(100%)
-
-
-
MSM
-
N = 4
(33%)
N = 5
(41%)
N = 2
(16%)
-
N = 2
(8%) (
1 MSR)
22. The
behavior of
making eye
contact with
a partner.
(NV) *
Panel
0
N = 1
(5%)
N = 7
(38%)
N = 9
(50%)*
0
N = 1
(5%)
SR
-
-
N = 2
(40%)
N = 3
(60%)
-
-
MSM
N = 1
(8%)
N = 5
(41%)
N = 6
(41%) ( 1
MSR)
-
N = 1
(8%)
23. The
behavior of
avoiding eye
contact with
a partner
Panel
0
0
N = 1
(5%)
N = 2
(11%)
N = 14
(77%)*
*
N = 1
(5%)
SR
-
-
-
N =1
(20%)
N = 4
(80%)
-
75
(NV)**
MSM
-
-
N = 1
(8%)
N = 1
(8%)
N = 10
(83%)
N = 1 (
1 MSR)
24. The
behavior of
turning away
from a
partner (NV)
**
Panel
0
0
0
N = 3
(16%)
N = 15
(83%)*
*
0
SR
-
-
-
-
N = 5
(100%)
-
MSM
-
-
-
N = 3
(25%)
N= 10
(75%)
( 1
MSR)
-
25. The
behavior of
one raising
their
eyebrows
while looking
at a partner
(NV)*
Panel
0
N = 6
(33%)
N = 2
(11%)
N = 9
(50%)*
0
N = 1
(5%)
SR
-
N =1
(20%)
-
N = 4
(80%)
-
-
MSM
-
N = 5
(33%) (
1 MSR)
N = 2
(16%)
N = 5
(41%)
-
N = 1
(8%)
26. The
behavior
breathing
heavily (NV)
**
Panel
N = 1
(5%)
0
N = 2
(11%)
N = 14
(77%)**
0
N = 1
(5%)
SR
N =1
(20%)
-
-
N = 4
(80%)
-
-
MSM
-
-
N = 2
(16%)
N= 10
(75%) ( 1
MSR)
-
N = 1
(8%)
27. The
behavior of
leaning into/
rubbing
against a
partner
(NV)*
Panel
0
N = 6
(33%)
N = 11
(61%) *
N = 1
(5%)
0
0
SR
-
N =1
(20%)
N = 3
(60%)
N =1
(20%)
-
-
MSM
-
N = 5
(41%)
N = 8
(58%) (1
MSR)
-
-
-
28. The
Panel
0
N = 6
N = 8
N = 3
0
N = 1
76
behavior of
sharing
provocative
content with
a partner
(NV)
(33%)
(44%)
(16%)
(5%)
SR
-
N = 2
(40%)
N = 3
(60%)
-
-
-
MSM
N = 4
(25%) (
1 MSR)
N = 5
(41%)
N = 3
(25%)
N = 1
(8%)
29. The
behavior of
showing a
partner one is
wearing
provocative
clothes or
accessories
(underwear,
harness, etc.)
*
Panel
0
N = 11
(61%)*
N = 5
(27%)
N = 2
(11%)
0
0
SR
-
N = 3
(60%)
N = 2
(40%)
-
-
-
MSM
-
N = 8
(58%) (
1 MSR)
N = 3
(25%)
N = 2
(16%)
-
-
*Note. Level of agreement * Endorsement = > 50% participant agreement ; ** Consensus =
>70% participant agreement *** Major Consensus = >90% participant agreement. When
applicable MSM Sexual Researchers are noted within totals as (# MSR)
77
DISCUSSION
The current study examined the concepts of sexual consent, sexual non-consent and
nonverbal sexual consent communication behaviors amongst a panel of experts. This study
utilized the Delphi Method (Dalkey, 1969), which is a multi-round mixed method approach to
research that provides structure for consumer/expert consensus (Jorm, 2015). In the context of
the current study, consumers were MSM community members and experts were researchers of
human sexuality, sexual violence, and sexual violence prevention (with approximately two
participants identifying as both). The current study is novel in sexual consent and sexual consent
communication behavior research due to its focus on the inclusion of MSM community members
which are historically underrepresented in studies of sexual consent and sexual consent
communication (Muehlenhard, Humphreys, Jozkowski & Peterson, 2016). This study sought to
explore the concepts of sexual consent, sexual non-consent, and nonverbal sexual consent
communication behaviors, as conceptualized by these groups.
Sexual Consent
The qualities of sexual consent were explored through all three rounds of this Delphi
Study. Participants provided and reviewed 31 descriptions of sexual consent and largely found
commonality around personal opinions regarding sexual consent. Utilizing a Likert scale,
participants were able to give numerical feedback regarding the adequacy of a peer’s description
of sexual consent. The results from the current study highlight that within this current group (The
Panel) and the individual stakeholders making up the group (SHS) individuals appear to be
largely in agreement about what does and does not constitute an adequate description of sexual
consent. Of the 31 descriptions provided by The Panel, individuals seemingly came to consensus
78
on the relevance of 24 descriptions (77%) describing the concept of sexual consent. Similar
studies in expert/ consumer research, note that such a consensus percentage is ideal when
attempting to further understand and develop culturally congruent interventions and guidelines
for nuanced topics such as post disaster psychosocial care protocols and the development of a
mental health first aid programs for Indigenous Australians (Bisson et al., 2010; Hart et al.
2009). Taken together, the current study suggests that The Panel’s findings may help further
define the relevance of a shared knowledge around the concept of what defines Sexual Consent.
Such information may be relevant when considering educational and preventative programing
around Sexual consent and sexual consent communication.
Regarding the descriptions of sexual consent provided by participants in Round One, The
Panel reached a level of consensus of over 90% (Major Consensus) on seven descriptions of
sexual consent provided by the individuals on the panel (descriptions 3, 4, 5,6,15, & 23). As
noted in the results, amongst the seven descriptions which achieved Major Consensus, three
qualities of sexual consent were commonly shared and mentioned specifically by participants in
qualitative feedback (Sexual consent should be freely/given without influence; Sexual consent
should be ongoing; Sexual consent should be permission granting/ affirming). Taken together the
strong qualitative and quantitative feedback suggest that participants both collectively and
individually, support the qualities that sexual consent should be freely given without coercion,
permission granting/ affirming and ongoing through an interaction. These qualities are shared
values of sexual violence prevention programs and aspirational models of sexual consent such as
affirmative consent (Soble, 2002). Therefore, these results suggest that across our panel, despite
differing sexual orientations, age, gender identities, and exposure to violence prevention
literature, The Panel shares a common understanding and shared appreciation for these qualities
79
of sexual consent. Our data suggests further exploration into the role of demographic features
such as gender identity and sexual orientation and their relationship to sexual consent
understanding may be warranted. Historically, other studies have cited factors such as gender
identity have been impactful when considering sexual consent and sexual consent
communication, our findings suggest further exploration of these demographic factors as well
additional factors ( e.g. age, educational achievement) may be relevant as well(Jozkowski et al.,
2017; Peplau, 2003).
Additionally, the cohesion amongst the panel that our results suggest may also speak to
the influence of sexual script theory on individuals understanding of and enactment of sexual
consent and sexual consent communication. For the current study, participants were sourced
from North America and were living and working in the United States and Canada at the time of
the study. Thus, the overall cohesion between SHS and the The Panel as a whole provide
additional support for the influence of common cultural scenarios in the process of developing
beliefs, attitudes, and expectations (intrapersonal scripts) regarding sexual consent and sexual
consent communication behaviors (Simon and Gagnon, 1986). Furthermore, relationship status
and duration of relationship have all been associated with impacting perceptions of sexual
consent (Humphreys, 2007). As ThePanel of the current study largely identified as being in a
committed monogamous relationship, it is plausible that these shared experiences also influenced
the level of cohesion amongst participants. Therefore, the results of the current study also may
lend support to examining the impacts of relationship status and duration of relationship on
sexual consent communication and conceptualization.
Based on the initial qualitative responses of both SHS groups and The Panel, six
categories of sexual consent qualities were derived by the researcher. These qualities of sexual
80
consent emphasize mutuality, confirmation of consent by verbal and nonverbal behaviors, and
the impact of inequity on consent between partners. The six qualities of sexual consent derived
were: (1) Sexual consent should be mutual between all parties (2) Sexual consent should be
permission granting/ affirming. (3) Sexual consent should be confirmed via verbal and non-
verbal behaviors. (4) Sexual consent should be freely/given without influence. (5) Sexual consent
should be ongoing. (6) Sexual consent should be reversible/ revocable. These findings from the
current study may lend to future research as the process of defining sexual consent amongst
individuals is novel. Within literature there have been instances of research authors have been
known to engage in “spontaneous consent”; that is, failing to define the subject (Beres, 2007).
Findings from the current study regarding the qualities of sexual consent also share a
similarities with with the seven tenants of affirmative consent (Antioch College, 2016). The
qualities derived in this study and the seven tenants of affirmative consent largely overlap,
especially in the notions of sexual consent being ongoing, permission granting/affirming and
given freely without coercion (Soble, 2002). Interestingly, The Panel differed from the tenants
in one key way which was they did not include the central tenant of affirmative consent, verbal
consent. More specifically, as a model of violence prevention, the Antioch College affirmative
consent model heavily emphasizes “if the level of sexual intimacy increases during an
interaction… the people involved need to express their clear verbal consent before moving to
that new level” (Soble, 2002, p. 327). Aspirationally, this tenant would mean that when
practiced, those engaging in sexual consent communication with affirmative consent would
confirm activity with verbal communication behavior-ideally an enthusiastic “yes!” (Affirmative
Consent and Respect., 2017). However, participants in the current study provided 31 descriptions
of sexual consent, which noted the importance of consent being “ongoing”, but they did not
81
reference a need for parties to engage in an overtly verbal consent behaviors during escalation of
sexual activity.
The qualities provided and affirmed by the Panel suggest that conceptually, despite
differences in demographic factors (e.g., sexual orientation, gender identity, SHS), the values and
qualities of sexual consent amongst the Panel are largely consistent with aspirational models of
sexual consent. These findings again suggest that there may be more of a common understanding
of sexual consentbased on shared cultural scripts, at least in response to formal questions--
among individuals who share a cultural script (Beres, 2007). However, more research across
different communities (even within the U.S.) is warranted. It is also worth noting that responding
to questions on an academic survey may be different then real-world sexual consent
communication between (potential) sexual partnersfor both expert and community SHS
members.
In Round Two and Three, participants were asked to review and rank order the six
qualities of sexual consent in order of importance. During these rankings more diverse thinking
was evident amongst The Panel. Specifically, when compared to their female-identified sexual
researcher counterparts, a majority of MSM members (and one sexual researcher who also
identified as MSM) endorsed Sexual Consent should be mutual (n=8) as most important by
placing it in the first rank order position. In contrast the female-identified sexual researchers,
noted the most important qualities to be Sexual consent should be permission granting (n=3)/
affirming and sexual consent should be freely given (n=2). Interestingly the value placed on
mutuality of sexual consent by male-identified participants, and permission granting by female
participants, are in line with the cultural scenario of men being “pleasure seekers” and women
being “gate keepers” (Hirsch et al., 2019; Jozkowski, 2013; 2015; Peplau 2003). Additionally,
82
during the initial ranking the quality of Sexual consent should be freely given had five
participants indicate it belonged in the sixth rank order position (the lowest position), this
number of participants doubled (n=10) in the second ranking of sexual consent qualities. Thus,
within the confines of the study, these results may provide some support for the impact of
cultural scenarios on personal beliefs around the value of some sexual consent qualities over
others.
Sexual Non-Consent
Reviewing sexual consent literature, relatively few studies look at or explicitly examine
sexual non-consent (Cook & Messman-Moore, 2018; Struckman-Johnson et al., 2003; McKie et
al., 2020). In the current study, sexual non-consent was explored through two of the rounds of
this Delphi study. Initially 20 descriptions of sexual non-consent were produced by participants
in round two and were rated by the Panel in the subsequent round three. Interestingly, of the 20
descriptions of sexual non-consent produced, 19 met a form of group agreement, with one
description meeting criterion for Major Consensus (>90%). Again, our results suggest a level of
uniformity amongst the panel which seemingly is novel when compared to other studies of
experiences of sexual consent communication (Hirsch et al., 2019; Jozkowski, 2013; King et al.
2020). However, these preliminary findings should be follow up in subsequent studies, with
larger and more diverse groups of participants to verify that the findings can persist.
The Panel expressed high levels of agreement amongst the quantitative data, thus when
examining the qualitative data a fuller picture of participants experience and thoughts regarding
sexual non-consent is painted. The strongest description produced by the panel was (as rated by
the Panel) was description 7 which read:
Not giving consent would include when not all parties agree to the sexual activity.
83
This non-consent can take the form of verbal and non-verbal cues (e.g., saying
"No," pushing someone away), reversing a decision for consent that may have
been given earlier, when an involved party is not an adult, or an adult in a power
dynamic not capable of consent in the first place (e.g., child, prisoner, mental
health considerations).
Description 7 (together with Description 8) also received strong qualitative feedback with
one MSM participant remarking: “No 7 and 8 are great examples because they also include
mental state (clouded by drugs or alcohol) and power dynamics as forcing non consent. Non
consent is more than just no”. The views expressed in this statement are meaningful when
relating back to larger concepts of both sexual violence prevention and sexual consent
communication. Critics of early sexual consent campaigns, such as the “no means no”
movements, have argued that the word “no” is an oversimplification—and not the only
indication--of non-consent (Kitzinger & Frith, 1999; Marcantonio & Jozkowski, 2020). Research
into sexual non-consent describe sexual non-consent as having multiple levels, including a lack
of desire, regret, social manipulation, and a violation of personal boundary (Cook & Messman-
Moore, 2018; Struckman-Johnson et al., 2003; McKie et al., 2020). As noted by the results
produced by The Panel in our current study, it appears The Panel’s perceptions of sexual non-
consent are in line with the literature on this topic.
Similarly, The Panel consistently noted the impact and need to speak to power dynamics
and other coercive forces when examining sexual non-consent. These results are line with
contemporary literature on sexual violence prevention that suggests the importance of power
differentials in non-consensual interactions. Specifically, gender dynamics, sexual minority
status, age, and intimidation have been noted as means of sexual exploitation in studies (Cook &
84
Messman-Moore, 2018; Rich, 1980; Struckman-Johnson et al., 2003). Thus, the results recorded
from this study, lend support to the practice of integrating these concepts (i.e., power
differentials and social dynamics) into sexual violence prevention at large. Furthermore, the
results from the current study may lend to further exploration around the concepts of extraneous
forces on the communication of sexual consent and sexual consent communication.
Interestingly, within our panel, there was some diversity in thought when comparing
stakeholder groups. Within the MSM SHS group qualitative comments revealed differences in
views of sexual non-consent that are not apparent in the quantitative data. In qualitative feedback
to descriptions 11 and 15:
(11) “Non-consent is any sexual action that occurs that is not agreed upon, occurs with a
power differential, one of the members is influenced by a substance, there is force, or finally that
consent is withdrawn but the action continues.” and (15)” … This can also include moments
where an individual is unable to provide consent, such as when someone is drinking alcohol or is
unconscious.”.
In response to these descriptions, one MSM participant noted that for him, the use of substances,
specifically alcohol, “does not automatically constitute sexual non-consent. The qualitative
feedback provided by this MSM member aligns with documented literature regarding some
sexual practices and beliefs within the MSM community regarding substance use during sexual
intercourse (Giorgetti, 2017; McKie et al., 2020; Palamar et al., 2014). However, in contrast to
this participant, other MSM participants within the current study noted the importance and
centrality of substance use and “mental state” when considering sexual non-consent and the
communication of sexual non consent. These conflicts regarding substance use and sexual
consent and sexual consent communication practices, has also been an area of concern with
85
researchers and advocates within MSM communities (Abbey, 1987; McKie et al., 2020;
Newcomb, 2014). Differences in these opposing opinions may reflect different cultural values
within our Panel of MSM participants and potentially the MSM community itself. Furthermore,
these differences may also reflect different values and preferences regarding those who engage
monogamous relationships (as our Panel members overwhelmingly were in monogamous
relationships) which has been noted in other studies to affect perception of sexual consent and its
communication (Humphreys, 2007).
Nonverbal Sexual Consent Communication Behaviors
A novel element of the current study is the examination of nonverbal sexual consent
communication behaviors, specifically the interpretation of nonverbal sexual consent behaviors.
To the authors knowledge, several studies exist examining the use of nonverbal sexual consent
communication behaviors in college-aged population, but this is the first study to examine these
behaviors outside of this group (Johnson & Hoover, 2015; Jozkowski & Humphreys, 2014).
Initially participants reviewed and provided qualitative feedback regarding Beres et al.’s (2007)
list of nonverbal sexual consent behaviors. Nineteen participants noted no changes or offered no
additional feedback to the list of nonverbal behaviors. When examining those 19, proportionally
MSM participants were less likely to offer no feedback when compared to their sexual researcher
peers.
As alluded to earlier, demographics of those in the sexual researchers group largely
identify as female compared to exclusively male-identified MSM group. This discrepancy in
commentary, therefore, speak to trend differences between these two groups to utilize nonverbal
sexual consent communication behaviors. These results seemingly support one trend in literature
which the gendered difference in level of value placed on nonverbal sexual consent
86
communication behaviors by male-identified individuals when compared to female-identified
individuals (Hall, 1998; Jozkowski & Peterson, 2013; King et al. 2020). Additionally, our
results mirror information in literature which highlights specific nonverbal sexual consent
communication behaviors observed within the MSM community. Particularly, several comments
made by MSM participants reference behaviors such as: eyeing a partner, standing in a urinal for
an unusually long time, flashing headlights, following a partner, leaving a shower door open,
passing a partner multiple times while making eye contact. These behaviors are in line with well
documented cruising behaviors which are common exchanges of nonverbal consent
communication amongst MSM (Frankis & Flowers, 2009; Mckie et al. 2020). Thus, when taken
together, the participation in offering nonverbal qualitative comments may offer additional. this
split amongst participants is notable as noted previous sections they have largely been high levels
of cohesion.
Nonverbal sexual consent communication behaviors were explored further in this study
as The Panel was tasked with sorting the expanded nonverbal communication behaviors list into
categories of usage. Current literature on nonverbal sexual consent communication behaviors is
mixed, as it is often reported there are gendered (heteronormative) experiences of interpretation
and subscription to use of nonverbal sexual consent communication behaviors to receive sexual
consent (King et al.2020; Righi et al., 2019; Willis & Jozkowski, 2019). However, the enactment
of nonverbal sexual consent communication behaviors to convey sexual consent is equally
preferred by both males and females (Hall, 1998; Curtis & Burnett, 2017; Marcantonio &
Jozkowski, 2020; Shumlich & Fisher, 2018). Thus, in the current study, twenty-nine nonverbal
sexual consent communication behaviors were sorted among a Panel of 18 individuals and two
SHS groups (sexual researchers and MSM). The nonverbal sexual consent communication
87
behaviors were sorted into six categories by the Panel (e.g., Consent Giving Behavior, a Consent
Seeking Behavior, an Interchangeable Behavior, Ambiguous behavior, a Refusal behavior or an
Unused Behavior). The Panel came to a level of agreement on 24 nonverbal sexual consent
communication behaviors and their usage. Interestingly, only one behavior “The behavior of
distancing onself physically from a partner, reached Major consensus with 95% of the panel
noting this nonverbal communication behavior was a Refusal behavior. This finding is in line
with much of the current literature on sexual consent communication (Marcantonio &
Jozkowski, 2020).
As noted, much of the current literature on sexual consent communication behaviors note
a gendered experience in the perception of and use of Sexual consent communication behaviors
(King et al.2020; Righi et al., 2019; Willis & Jozkowski, 2019). Thus, when examining the data
provided by The Panel, an examination of the possibility of gendered experiences of nonverbal
sexual communication was done via the use of a chi square test of independence. Utilizing a chi
square test of independence between two groups (Sexual Researchers and MSM [including 1
MSR]) no behaviors were determined to operate independently, meaning we were unable to
reject the null hypothesis and thus it appears that an individual’s identity did not impact their
sorting of nonverbal sexual consent communication behaviors. However, when examining the
data amongst three groups (Sexual Researchers, MSM, and MSR) four nonverbal behaviors were
found to have a p value >.05 and thus the null hypothesis was able to be rejected. This again
suggests that there may be more information to be explored when considering a larger more
diverse group of individuals. Follow-up studies that take a more in depth look at how nonverbal
consent behaviors are made and interpreted within the MSM community are warranted, as the
current data seems to only begin to suggest that important differences exist in the areas
88
nonverbal sexual consent behaviors.
Overall, the implications of these results suggest what is largely documented in research
in college-aged populations, that nonverbal sexual consent communication behaviors exist and
are utilized by sexually active individuals (Humphreys & Brousseau, 2010; Humphreys &
Herold, 2007; Jozkowski & Peterson, 2013; King et al. 2020). These results are also novel in the
fact that the indicate while nonverbal consent behaviors are being used by a variety of
individuals, and many of those behaviors were categorized were categorized similarly across
both researchers (mostly female identified) and community members (mostly male identified).
Additionally, only a small number of non-verbal consent behaviors were distinctly identified as
overtly indicative of consent or non-consent (most other behaviors were identified as
interactional or ambiguous). Consequently, the need to have communities explore just want
particular non-verbal consent behaviors mean, and expanding the repertoire of those behaviors,
may be an important next step in sexual consent research.
Strengths and Limitations
The current study boasts several strengths, including its mixed methodological approach
to examining complex topics like sexual consent and sexual non-consent (Jorm, 2015). The use
of open-ended questions, open and axial coding, as well quantitative measurements allowed for
richer and deeper insight into the Panel’s experience with these topics. Furthermore, the diverse
make up of sexual orientations, gender identities, and ages, of participants of this study are
unique to sexual consent research reviewed and should be considered a strength of this study.
While the study as several strengths, including strengths related to the Delphi method, it
is not without limitations. For example, as with most elements of group consensus research, the
current study was structured around a form of “group think”. Thus, when considering such broad
89
ranging topics such as sexual consent, sexual non-consent, and nonverbal sexual consent
communication behaviors, it becomes very important to note who constitutes that group. In the
current study, there was a relative lack of racial, ethnic, consensually non-monogamous, and
educational diversity amongst the participants, which certainly impacted the data and outcomes.
This study also used a snowballing method of recruitment, and once identified participants had
an option to nominate an individual who they felt also met criteria for inclusion. Therefore, the
trustworthiness and generalizability of these results may be limited as there may be self-selection
bias and an inadvertent silo as a result of the snowball methodology employed. Finally, this
study looked at broad ranging topics, thus could have provided opportunities for deeper
reflection on specific elements of this area. Specifically, participants could have explored
nonverbal sexual consent communication behaviors in more depth had they had the opportunity
to review and provide feedback to other participants sorting.
Implications for Violence Prevention and Practice
The findings of the current study have implications for a variety of stakeholders and
future directions in research. First and foremost, the results of the current study demonstrate a
surprising level of cohesion, even across participants of various educational, gender and sexual
identities. Compared to past research, which suggests gender differences regarding views and
experiences of sexual consent and sexual consent communication (Jozkowski & Peterson, 2013;
King et. al, 2020; Peplau, 2003), the panel in this study showed high levels of cohesions across
tasks. Taken together, the findings are intriguing and suggest there may be more benefits
centered around this topic examining inter-group similarities around the concepts of sexual
consent communication. This may be a result of the age or educational status of participants in
this study, none-the-less these findings provide a platform to begin to consider what components
90
of sexual consent communication may be more common across communities, and which may be
unique to specific communities.
Regarding the concept of sexual consent and sexual non-consent, the findings of this
study bring about additional questions around what notions of sexual consent and sexual non-
consent are held in esteem by individuals. Our panel showed some interest that the core notions
of mutuality, lack of coercion, and ongoing consent were central across the Panel and other
models of consent boasted in research (Soble, 2002). Also noteworthy is the absence of verbal
consent communication behaviors as being the absolute in the consent communication process,
especially regarding confirming sexual consent is present through the duration of an encounter.
Furthermore, the notion of sexual non-consent is more than just a verbal “no” is apparent
throughout the Panel members. Sexual non-consent in general, and non-verbal non-consent
communication behaviors, warrants much more researchespecially within and across
communities. Studies that seek to understand how consent (and non-consent) are communicated
non-verbally are imperative if we are to truly prevent sexual violence and promote sex positivity
across communities. These studies are especially needed amongst underrepresented communities
in psychological and violence prevention research including and not limited to: black, indigenous
people of color (BIPOC), the educationally diverse including non-college, and the consensually
non-monogamous.
This study also noted the impacts of sub-culture membership such as MSM may
influence views in these two areasas does previous literature in the area (Beres, & MacDonald,
2015; Frankis & Flowers, 2005; Pitagora, 2013). Further research within non-majority based,
cultures may be warranted to help examine the impact of sub cultural behaviors on interpersonal
scripts and intrapersonal beliefs related to sexual consent in the context of substance use and
91
non-monogamous sexual interactions. Providing safety standards, especially in the area
recreation drug use, may also be an important point research agenda related to sexual consent.
Perhaps the most important, though incomplete, implication of this study is just how little
is known and understood about non-verbal consent behaviors. Clearly, non-verbal consent
behaviors are central to the lived experiences, and research understanding, of our Panel Members
(both MSM and Researchers). There was also a significant amount of agreement about how to
categorize specific non-verbal consent behaviors (e.g., consent-giving, consent requesting,
ambiguous, non-consenting). However, because the amount of behaviors that were clearly
categorized as consent-giving is extremely small, we argue that both research and community
campaigns are need to provide better understanding and community norms in this area. In terms
of research, we argue that it is imperative to gain knowledge on how people from different
communities and identities give consent non-verbally, how that message received (encoded) by
others, and how non-verbal consent encoding also differs by community, gender (and gender
scripts; citation), and type of sexual interaction. It is only with better understanding, followed by
intentional positive norm setting by community stake-holders, that we might make progress
toward a more healthy sexual consent communication strategies. Furthermore, as this is overall a
smaller subsection of a group, more follow up with larger and even more diverse participants is
warranted.
Conclusion
In conclusion, the current study engaged in a consumer/expert exploration of sexual
consent amongst two SHS groups: MSM and sexual researchers. The current study sought to add
to the literature in this field, and particularly address a gap in the literature regarding an
understudied and complex topic, sexual consent, sexual non-consent, and nonverbal sexual
92
communication behaviors. The findings of the current study reveal despite differing sexual
orientations, gender identities, and experiences with sexual violence prevention literature, largely
the Panel shared common views regarding these topics. Furthermore, the group was able to
highlight the diversity of their views regarding sexual consent communication and specifically
nonverbal sexual consent communication behaviors.
Literature supports education and conversation around sexual consent and verbal sexual
consent communication behaviors are an effective means of supporting sexual violence
prevention. However, the findings of the current study highlight the need to broaden the scope
this conversation and encourage further research and attention to the presence and impact of
nonverbal sexual consent communication behaviors in lived experiences of individuals.
Furthermore, the complex results of this study highlight the need to work on and explore sexual
consent communication practices with a wider variety of sexually active individuals to better
understand these practices outside of the collegiate atmosphere.
93
References
Abbey, A. (1982). Sex differences in attributions for friendly behavior: Do males misperceive females'
friendliness? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 42(5), 830-838.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.42.5.830
Abbey, A. (1987). Misperceptions of Friendly Behavior as Sexual Interest: A Survey of Naturally
Occurring Incidents. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 11(2), 173-194. doi:10.1111/j.1471-
6402.1987.tb00782.x
Abbey, A., Ross, L. T., Mcduffie, D., & Mcauslan, P. (1996). Alcohol and Dating Risk Factors for
Sexual Assault Among College Women. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 20(1), 147-169.
doi:10.1111/j.1471-6402.1996.tb00669.x
Affirmative Consent and Respect. (2017). Affirmative consent campus policy report. Retrieved
from http://affirmativeconsent.com/consentpolicy/
Airey, J. L. (2018). #MeToo. Tulsa Studies in Women's Literature 37(1), 7-13. The University of Tulsa.
Retrieved April 28, 2019, from Project MUSE database. doi:10.1353/tsw.2018.0000
Ali, R. (2011). Dear colleague letter from assistant secretary for civil rights Russlynn Ali.
Washington, DC: Department of Education Office for Civil Rights, U.S. Retrieved from
http://www2.ed.gov/about/offices/list/ocr/letters/colleague-201104.html
Antioch College. Sexual Offense Prevention Policy (SOPP) overview. 2016. Available online:
https://www.antiochcollege.edu/sites/default/files/04.015_Sexual-Offense-Prevention-
Policy.pdf.
Association of American Universities. (2015). Climate Survey on Sexual Assault and Sexual
94
Misconduct. Retrieved from https://www.aau.edu/key-issues/aau-climate-survey-sexual-
assault-and-sexual-misconduct-2015
Beres, M. A. (2007). ‘Spontaneous’ Sexual Consent: An Analysis of Sexual Consent
Literature. Feminism & Psychology, 17(1), 93-108. doi:10.1177/0959353507072914
Beres, M. A. (2014). Rethinking the concept of consent for anti-sexual violence activism and
education. Feminism & Psychology, 24(3), 373-389. doi:10.1177/0959353514539652
Beres, M. A., Herold, E., & Maitland, S. B. (2004). Sexual Consent Behaviors in Same-Sex
Relationships. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 33(5), 475-486.
doi:10.1023/b:aseb.0000037428.41757.10
Beres, M., & MacDonald, J. (2015). Talking About Sexual Consent: Heterosexual women and
BDSM. Australian Feminist Studies, 30(86), 418432.
https://doi.org/10.1080/08164649.2016.1158692
Beckmann, A. (2003). “Sexual rights” and “sexual responsibilities” within consensual “S/M” practice.
In M. Cowling & P. Reynolds (Eds.), Making sense of sexual consent (pp. 195208).
Burlington, VT: Ashgate.
Block, M. (2004). Consent. In M. D. Smith (Ed.), Encyclopedia of rape
(pp. 5152). Westport, CT: Greenwood Press.
Bowen, A. (2005). Internet sexuality research with rural men who have sex with men: Can
we recruit and retain them? Journal of Sex Research, 42(4), 317-323.
doi:10.1080/00224490509552287
Bullock, D. (2004). Lesbian Cruising: An Examination of the Concept and Methods. Journal of
Homosexuality, 47(2), 131. https://doi.org/10.1300/J082v47n02_01
95
Camilleri, J. A., Quinsey, V. L., & Tapscott, J. L. (2008). Assessing the Propensity for Sexual Coaxing
and Coercion in Relationships: Factor Structure, Reliability, and Validity of the Tactics to
Obtain Sex Scale. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 38(6), 959-973. doi:10.1007/s10508-008-9377-
2
Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. (2017). Youth Risk Behavior Surveillance- United States
2017. Retrieved from https://www.cdc.gov/healthyyouth/data/yrbs/pdf/2017/ss6708.pdf
Cook, N., & Messman-Moore, T. (2018). I Said No: The Impact of Voicing Non-Consent on Women’s
Perceptions of and Responses to Rape. Violence Against Women, 24(5), 507527.
https://doi.org/10.1177/1077801217708059
Curtis, J. N., & Burnett, S. (2017). Affirmative Consent: What Do College Student Leaders Think
About “Yes Means Yes” as the Standard for Sexual Behavior? American Journal of Sexuality
Education, 12(3), 201-214. doi:10.1080/15546128.2017.1328322
Dalkey, O. (1969). THE DELPHI METHOD: AN EXPERIMENTAL STUDY OF GROUP OPINION.
de la Ossa, A. C. (2016). ‘Talk, listen, think’: Discourses of agency and unintentional violence in
consent guidance for gay, bisexual and trans men. Discourse & Society, 27(4), 365-382.
doi:10.1177/0957926516634549
Decker, J. F., & Baroni, P. G. (2011). "No" still means "yes": the failure of the "non-consent" reform
movement in American rape and sexual assault law. Journal of Criminal Law and Criminology,
101(4), 1081+.
https://link.gale.com/apps/doc/A283591886/EAIM?u=ndacad_58202zund&sid=bookmark-
EAIM&xid=7f42d44f
De León, S., Jackson, S., Lowenthal, A., Ammiano, A., Beall, S., Cannella, S., ... & Wolk, S. (2014)
Senate Bill 967- Student safety: Sexual assault. California Legislative Information. Retrieved
96
from https://leginfo.legislature.ca.gov/faces/billNavClient.xhtmlDworkin, S. L., &
O'Sullivan, L. (2005). Actual versus desired initiation patterns among a sample of college men:
Tapping disjunctures within traditional male sexual scripts. Journal of Sex Research, 42(2), 150-
158. doi:10.1080/00224490509552268
Edgar, T., & Fitzpatrick, M. A. (1993). Expectations for sexual interaction: A cognitive test of
the sequencing of sexual communication behaviors. Health Communication, 5, 239261.
https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327027hc0504_1
Fantasia, H. C. (2011). Really not even a decision any more. Journal of Forensic Nursing, 7(3), 120-
129. doi:10.1111/j.1939-3938.2011.01108.x
Frankis, J., & Flowers, P. (2009). Public Sexual Cultures: A Systematic Review of Qualitative Research
Investigating Men’s Sexual Behaviors with Men in Public Spaces. Journal of
Homosexuality, 56(7), 861893. https://doi.org/10.1080/00918360903187846
Forbes, A., & Hutchison, A. (2020). Counseling Competencies for the English-Speaking
Caribbean. The Counseling Psychologist, 48(5), 657684.
https://doi.org/10.1177/0011000020911007
Hall, D.S. (1998) Consent for Sexual Behavior in a College Student Population. Electronic Journal of
Human Sexuality 1. www.ejhs.org
Halley, J. (2016). The Move to Affirmative Consent. Signs: Journal of Women in Culture and
Society, 42(1), 257279. https://doi.org/10.1086/686904
Hasson, F., Keeney, S., & McKenna, H. (2000). Research guidelines for the Delphi survey
technique. Journal of Advanced Nursing, 32(4), 10081015. https://doi.org/10.1046/j.1365-
2648.2000.01567.x
97
Hickman, S. E., & Muehlenhard, C. L. (1999). “By the semi‐mystical appearance of a condom”: How
young women and men communicate sexual consent in heterosexual situations. Journal of Sex
Research, 36(3), 258-272. doi:10.1080/00224499909551996
Hirsch, J., Khan, S., Wamboldt, A., & Mellins, C. (2019). Social Dimensions of Sexual Consent
Among Cisgender Heterosexual College Students: Insights From Ethnographic
Research. Journal of Adolescent Health, 64(1), 2635.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jadohealth.2018.06.011
Chia-Chien Hsu, & Brian A. Sandford. (2007). The Delphi Technique: Making Sense of
Consensus. Practical Assessment, Research & Evaluation, 12(10), 18.
Forbes, A., & Hutchison, A. (2020). Counseling Competencies for the English-Speaking
Caribbean. The Counseling Psychologist, 48(5), 657684.
https://doi.org/10.1177/0011000020911007
Humphreys, T. (2007). Perceptions of Sexual Consent: The Impact of Relationship History and
Gender. Journal of Sex Research, 44(4), 307-315. doi:10.1080/00224490701586706
Humphreys, T. P., & Brousseau, M. M. (2010). The sexual consent scale-revised: Development,
reliability, and preliminary validity. Journal of Sex Research, 47, 420428.
doi:10.1080/00224490903151358
Humphreys, T. P., & Herold, E. (2007). Sexual Consent in Heterosexual Relationships: Development of
a New Measure. Sex Roles, 57, 305315.doi:10.1007/s11199-007-9264-7
Javaid, A. (2018). Out of place: Sexualities, sexual violence, and heteronormativity. Aggression and
Violent Behavior, 39, 83-89. doi:10.1016/j.avb.2018.02.007
98
Johnson, A. M., & Hoover, S. M. (2015). The potential of sexual consent interventions on college
campuses: A literature review on the barriers to establishing affirmative sexual consent. Pure
Insights, 4(5), 14.
Jorm AF. Using the Delphi expert consensus method in mental health research. Australian & New
Zealand Journal of Psychiatry. 2015;49(10):887-897. doi:10.1177/0004867415600891
Jozkowski, K. N. (2015). “Yes Means Yes”? Sexual Consent Policy and College Students. Change:
The Magazine of Higher Learning, 47(2), 16-23. doi:10.1080/00091383.2015.1004990
Jozkowski KN and Humphreys TP, Sexual consent on college campuses: implications for sexual assault
prevention education, Health Education Monograph Series, 2014, 31 ( 2 ): 30 36
Jozkowski, K. N., Marcantonio, T. L., & Hunt, M. E. (2017). College Students Sexual Consent
Communication And Perceptions of Sexual Double Standards: A Qualitative Investigation.
Perspectives on Sexual and Reproductive Health, 49(4), 237-244. doi:10.1363/psrh.12041
Jozkowski, K. N., & Peterson, Z. D. (2013). College Students and Sexual Consent: Unique
Insights. Journal of Sex Research, 50(6), 517-523. doi:10.1080/00224499.2012.700739
Jozkowski, K. N., Peterson, Z. D., Sanders, S. A., Dennis, B., & Reece, M. (2013). Gender Differences
in Heterosexual College Students' Conceptualizations and Indicators of Sexual Consent:
Implications for Contemporary Sexual Assault Prevention Education. The Journal of Sex
Research, 51(8), 904-916. doi:10.1080/00224499.2013.792326
Jozkowski, K. N., Sanders, S., Peterson, Z. D., Dennis, B., & Reece, M. (2014). Consenting to Sexual
Activity: The Development and Psychometric Assessment of Dual Measures of
Consent. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 43(3), 437-450. doi:10.1007/s10508-013-0225-7
King, B., Fallon, M., Reynolds, E., Williamson, K., Barber, A., & Giovinazzo, A. (2020). College
Students’ Perceptions of Concurrent/Successive Nonverbal Behaviors as Sexual
99
Consent. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 886260520905544886260520905544.
https://doi.org/10.1177/0886260520905544
Kitzinger, C., & Frith, H. (1999). Just Say No? The Use of Conversation Analysis in Developing a
Feminist Perspective on Sexual Refusal. Discourse & Society, 10(3), 293
316. https://doi.org/10.1177/0957926599010003002
Klinkenberg, D., & Rose, S. (1994). Dating scripts of gay men and lesbians. Journal of Homosexuality,
26, 2335. DOI: 10.1300/J082v26n04_02
Kosciw, J. G., Greytak, E. A., Giga, N. M., Villenas, C. & Danischewski, D. J. (2016). The 2015
National School Climate Survey: The experiences of lesbian, gay, bisexual, transgender, and
queer youth in our nation’s schools. New York: GLSEN.
Linstone, H., & Turoff, M. (1975). The Delphi method : techniques and applications . Addison-Wesley
Pub. Co., Advanced Book Program.
Marcantonio, T., & Jozkowski, K. (2020). Assessing How Gender, Relationship Status, and Item
Wording Influence Cues Used by College Students to Decline Different Sexual Behaviors. The
Journal of Sex Research, 57(2), 260272. https://doi.org/10.1080/00224499.2019.1659218
McCormick, N. B. (1979). Come-ons and Put-offs: Unmarried Students' Strategies for Having and
Avoiding Sexual Intercourse. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 4(2), 194-211.
doi:10.1111/j.1471-6402.1979.tb00708.x
McCormick, N. B. (1987). Sexual Scripts: Social and therapeutic implications. Sexual and Marital
Therapy, 2(1), 3-27. doi:10.1080/02674658708407734
McKie, R., Skakoon-Sparling, S., Levere, D., Sezlik, S., & Humphreys, T. (2020). Is There Space for
Our Stories? An Examination of North American and Western European Gay, Bi, and Other
100
Men Who Have Sex with Men’s Non-consensual Sexual Experiences. The Journal of Sex
Research, 57(8), 10141025. https://doi.org/10.1080/00224499.2020.1767023
Muehlenhard, C. L., Humphreys, T. P., Jozkowski, K. N., & Peterson, Z. D. (2016). The Complexities
of Sexual Consent Among College Students: A Conceptual and Empirical Review. The Journal
of Sex Research, 53(4-5), 457-487. doi:10.1080/00224499.2016.1146651
Nair, R., Aggarwal, R., & Khanna, D. (2011). Methods of Formal Consensus in
Classification/Diagnostic Criteria and Guideline Development. Seminars in Arthritis and
Rheumatism, 41(2), 95105. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.semarthrit.2010.12.001
Newcomb, M. (2014). Prevalence and patterns of smoking, alcohol use, and illicit drug use in young
men who have sex with men. Drug and Alcohol Dependence., 141.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2014.05.005
Orchowski, L. M., Barnett, N. P., Berkowitz, A., Borsari, B., Oesterle, D., & Zlotnick, C. (2018).
Sexual Assault Prevention for Heavy Drinking College Men: Development and Feasibility of an
Integrated Approach. Violence Against Women, 24(11), 1369
1396. https://doi.org/10.1177/1077801218787928
Ortiz-Torres, B., Williams, S., & Ehrhardt, A. (2003). Urban women’s gender scripts: Implications for
HIV prevention. Culture, Health & Sexuality, 5(1), 117. https://doi.org/10.1080/713804639
Palamar, J. J. , Kiang, M. V. , Storholm, E. D. , & Halkitis, P. N. (2014). A qualitative descriptive study
of perceived sexual effects of club drug use in gay and bisexual men. Psychology & Sexuality ,
5(2), 143160. https://doi-org.ezproxy.library.und.edu/10.1080/19419899.2012.679363
Parsons, J. T., Starks, T. J., Gamarel, K. E., & Grov, C. (2012). Non-monogamy and sexual relationship
quality among same-sex male couples. Journal of Family Psychology, 26(5), 669-677.
doi:10.1037/a0029561
101
Peplau, Letitia Anne. (2003). Human Sexuality: How Do Men and Women Differ? Current Directions
in Psychological Science: a Journal of the American Psychological Society, 12(2), 3740.
https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-8721.01221
Pitagora, D. (2013). Consent vs. coercion: BDSM interactions highlight a fine but immutable line. New
School Psychology Bulletin, 10(1), 2736.
Pugh, B., & Becker, P. (2018). Exploring definitions and prevalence of verbal sexual coercion and its
relationship to consent to unwanted sex: Implications for affirmative consent standards on
college campuses. Behavioral Sciences. 8(8), 69; doi:10.3390/bs8080069
Raffaele Giorgetti, Adriano Tagliabracci, Fabrizio Schifano, Simona Zaami, Enrico Marinelli, &
Francesco Paolo Busardò. (2017). When “Chems” Meet Sex: A Rising Phenomenon Called
“ChemSex.” Current Neuropharmacology, 15(5), 762770.
https://doi.org/10.2174/1570159X15666161117151148
Rich, A. (1980). Compulsory Heterosexuality and Lesbian Existence. Signs, 5(4), 631-660. Retrieved
from http://www.jstor.org/stable/3173834
Righi, M. K., Bogen, K. W., Kuo, C., & Orchowski, L. M. (2019). A qualitative analysis of beliefs
about sexual consent among high school students. Journal of Interpersonal Violence. Advance
online publication. https://doi.org/10.1177/ 0886260519842855
ROSE, S., & FRIEZE, I. H. (1989). YOUNG SINGLES’ SCRIPTS FOR A FIRST DATE. Gender &
Society, 3(2), 258268. https://doi.org/10.1177/089124389003002006
Rosenthal, D., Gifford, S., & Moore, S. (1998). Safe sex or safe love: Competing discourses? AIDS
Care, 10(1), 3547. https://doi.org/10.1080/09540129850124569
102
Ross, A., Kelly, C., & Jorm, A. (2014). Re-development of mental health first aid guidelines for
suicidal ideation and behaviour: a Delphi study. BMC Psychiatry, 14(1), 241241.
https://doi.org/10.1186/s12888-014-0241-8
Ross, M. W., Tikkanen, R., & Mansson, S. A. (2000). Differences between Internet samples and
conventional samples of men who have sex with men: Implications for research and HIV
interventions. Social Science and Medicine, 51, 749758.
Rule, N., & Alaei, R. (2016). Gaydar: The Perception of Sexual Orientation From Subtle Cues. Current
Directions in Psychological Science : a Journal of the American Psychological Society, 25(6),
444448. https://doi.org/10.1177/0963721416664403
Schank, R. C., & Abelson, R.P. (1977). Scripts, Plans, Goals and Understanding: an inquiry into
human knowledge structures. Hillsdale, New Jersey, Lawernce Erlbaum.
Shumlich, E., & Fisher, W. (2018). Affirmative sexual consent? Direct and unambiguous consent is
rarely included in discussions of recent sexual interactions. The Canadian Journal of Human
Sexuality, 27(3), 248260. https://doi.org/10.3138/cjhs.2017-0040
Simms, D. C., & Byers, E. S. (2012). Heterosexual Daters’ Sexual Initiation Behaviors: Use of the
Theory of Planned Behavior. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 42(1), 105-116. doi:10.1007/s10508-
012-9994-7
Simon, W., & Gagnon, J. H. (1986). Sexual scripts: Permanence and change. Archives of Sexual
Behavior, 15(2), 97-120. doi:10.1007/bf01542219
Simon, W., & Gagnon, J. H. (2003). Sexual Scripts: Origins, Influences and Changes. Qualitative
Sociology, 26(4), 491-497. doi:10.1023/b:quas.0000005053.99846.e5
Skulmoski, Hartman, & Krahn, (2007). The Delphi Method for Graduate Research. Journal of
Information Technology Education, 6, 121. https://doi.org/10.28945/199
103
Smith, S.G., Zhang, X., Basile, K.C., Merrick, M.T., Wang, J., Kresnow, M., Chen, J. (2018). The
National Intimate Partner and Sexual Violence Survey (NISVS): 2015 Data Brief Updated
Release. Atlanta, GA: National Center for Injury Prevention and Control, Centers for Disease
Control and Prevention.
Soble, A. (2002). The philosophy of sex: Contemporary readings. Totowa, NJ: Rowman and
Littlefield.
Struckman-Johnson, C., Struckman-Johnson, D., & Anderson, P. B. (2003). Tactics of Sexual Coercion
Measure. PsycTESTS Dataset. doi:10.1037/t62165-000
Tewksbury, R. (1996). Cruising for sex in public places: The structure and language of men’s hidden,
erotic worlds. Deviant Behavior, 17(1), 119. https://doi.org/10.1080/01639625.1996.9968012
Ward, R. M., Weiner, J., Matthews, M. R., Hogan, K. M., & Popson, H. C. (2012). Alcohol and Sexual
Consent Scale. PsycTESTS Dataset. doi:10.1037/t40728-000
Wiederman, M. W. (2015). Sexual Script Theory: Past, Present, and Future. Handbooks of Sociology
and Social Research, 7-22. doi:10.1007/978-3-319-17341-2_2
Willis, M., Jozkowski, K., & Read, J. (2019). Sexual consent in K-12 sex education: an analysis of
current health education standards in the United States. Sex Education, 19(2), 226236.
https://doi.org/10.1080/14681811.2018.1510769
Willis, M., Jozkowski, K.N. Sexual Precedent’s Effect on Sexual Consent Communication. Arch Sex
Behav 48, 17231734 (2019). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10508-018-1348-7
Willis, M., Canan, S., Jozkowski, K., & Bridges, A. (2020). Sexual Consent Communication in Best-
Selling Pornography Films: A Content Analysis. The Journal of Sex Research, 57(1), 5263.
https://doi.org/10.1080/00224499.2019.1655522
104
APPENDIX A
Contingency Tables For significant sorted nonverbal sexual consent communication behaviors
during (SR x MSM x MSR) X
2
11. The behavior of letting a partner take one’s clothes off. (NV)*
SHS
Consent
Seeking
Consent
Giving
Interchangeable
Ambiguous
Declined
to Sort
Total
1 SRS
2
0
1
2
0
5
2 MSM
7
1
2
2
0
12
3 MSR
0
0
0
0
1
1
X
2
Tests
Value
Df
P
X
2
19.6
8
0.0012
N
18
105
15. The behavior of helping a partner undress oneself. (NV)
SHS
Consent
Seeking
Consent
Giving
Interchangeable
Ambiguous
Declined
to Sort
Total
1 SRS
0
3
2
0
0
5
2 MSM
2
4
5
1
0
12
3 MSR
0
0
0
0
1
1
X
2
Tests
Value
Df
P
X
2
22.6
8
0.004
N
18
106
21. The behavior of touching oneself. (NV) *
SHS
Consent
Seeking
Consent
Giving
Interchangeable
Ambiguous
Declined
to Sort
Total
1 SRS
0
0
5
0
0
5
2 MSM
4
0
5
2
1
12
3 MSR
0
0
10
0
1
1
X
2
Tests
Value
Df
P
X
2
13.5
6
0.036
N
18
107
23. The behavior of avoiding eye contact with a partner (NV)**
SHS
Consent
Seeking
Consent
Giving
Interchangeable
Ambiguous
Declined
to Sort
Refusal
Total
1 SRS
0
0
1
0
4
5
2 MSM
1
1
1
1
10
12
3 MSR
0
0
0
1
0
1
X
2
Tests
Value
Df
P
X
2
18.9
6
0.004
N
18